Wydrick Phillips v. Jiminez Allen , 668 F.3d 912 ( 2012 )


Menu:
  •                               In the
    United States Court of Appeals
    For the Seventh Circuit
    No. 10-3559
    W YDRICK P HILLIPS,
    Plaintiff-Appellant,
    v.
    JIMENEZ A LLEN, et al.,
    Defendants-Appellees.
    Appeal from the United States District Court
    for the Northern District of Illinois, Eastern Division.
    No. 07 C 666—Robert M. Dow, Jr., Judge.
    A RGUED N OVEMBER 2, 2011—D ECIDED F EBRUARY 10, 2012
    Before E ASTERBROOK, Chief Judge, and P OSNER and
    W OOD , Circuit Judges.
    E ASTERBROOK, Chief Judge. Ruby Graham cashed some
    checks at a currency exchange and with $5,000 in her
    purse drove to the public library in Bellwood, Illinois.
    She was accompanied by her mother, Elizabeth Graham.
    Just as Ruby stepped through the library’s door, her
    purse was grabbed from behind. Ruby turned and strug-
    gled with the snatcher. Elizabeth saw what was hap-
    2                                              No. 10-3559
    pening, yelled “no, no, not my baby,” and ran to give
    aid. The robber shot Elizabeth in the chest, then shot
    Ruby in the head, ran to the car, grabbed Elizabeth’s
    purse, and fled. Elizabeth was seriously injured, but
    Ruby suffered only a graze to her temple. Both were
    taken to the hospital. Ruby was released after receiving
    a few staples.
    Ruby described the attacker to two police offi-
    cers—one at the library, another at the hospital. She
    did not tell either officer that she had known her assail-
    ant. The day after the robbery, officer Jimenez Allen
    went to Elizabeth’s hospital room to question her. Allen
    found Ruby, along with James Bufkin, visiting Elizabeth.
    Bufkin told Allen that he had heard a rumor that
    Wydrick Phillips, who lived in the Grahams’ neighbor-
    hood, had been staking out currency exchanges and
    robbing people who cashed tax-refund checks (as Ruby
    had done). Later that day Ruby examined photographs
    at the police station to see whether she could identify
    the robber. All photographs had been chosen to meet
    the descriptions Ruby gave. She spent about 15 minutes
    examining five or six sheets of photographs, each con-
    taining six pictures (but no names). She concentrated on
    the fourth sheet. She told Allen that she knew the
    sixth person on the sheet but did not think him the robber.
    Ruby also told Allen that the first photo on this sheet
    was similar to the robber—but eventually she selected
    the fifth person on this page and told Allen that she
    was “sure” that he was the culprit. Ruby had selected
    a photograph of Wydrick Phillips.
    No. 10-3559                                                 3
    Police arrested Phillips, who was charged with robbery
    and attempted murder. He was acquitted when no evi-
    dence corroborated the testimony of Ruby and Elizabeth
    (both of whom picked Phillips from a lineup and
    identified him at trial). A search of Phillips’s home did
    not turn up any proceeds or weapons; a test of the
    clothing that he had been wearing on the day of the
    robbery was negative for gunpowder residue; and the
    defense introduced a manifest of FedEx deliveries
    showing (if it was accurate) that Phillips, a courier for that
    company, could not have been at the Bellwood library
    when the robbery and shootings occurred. Phillips then
    filed this suit under 42 U.S.C. §1983 against the Village
    of Bellwood and seven police officers who had par-
    ticipated in the investigation and prosecution, con-
    tending that he had been arrested without probable
    cause. The district court granted summary judgment to
    the defendants on this federal claim and relinquished
    supplemental jurisdiction of all state-law claims. 
    743 F. Supp. 2d 931
    (N.D. Ill. 2010). The judge concluded
    that Ruby’s selection of Phillips’s picture established
    probable cause for his arrest.
    Phillips’s principal argument is that Allen spoiled
    the identification procedure by speaking with Bufkin
    where Ruby could overhear, which primed her to
    finger Phillips. This made her identification unreliable,
    Phillips insists. He advances several other argu-
    ments—such as a contention that the police should
    have done more to follow up other leads before arresting
    him. (Police received tips implicating three other per-
    sons. They conducted a second photo array including
    4                                              No. 10-3559
    a picture of one of these three; Ruby did not select it.
    They did not show her pictures of the other two.) Phillips
    also contends that police should not have arrested
    him before his mother had a chance to show them a
    copy of the FedEx manifest that was produced as an
    alibi at trial. These are weak arguments. Police need not
    run down all leads before making an arrest—especially
    not when a crime is violent and leaving the perpetrator
    at large may endanger other persons. Nor need police
    wait for alibis, which even when presented they need
    not believe. A delivery manifest could be fabricated, or
    the times and locations of deliveries could be altered
    before the document is shown to police. See Hernandez v.
    Sheahan, 
    455 F.3d 772
    (7th Cir. 2006) (presenting prison
    officials with a document supposedly demonstrating
    innocence does not require the prisoner’s immediate
    release). Probable cause is established by a reasonable
    belief that a person committed a crime. See Illinois v.
    Gates, 
    462 U.S. 213
    (1983). Police are entitled to leave to
    the criminal process the full examination of potential
    defenses. See, e.g., Hebron v. Touhy, 
    18 F.3d 421
    (7th Cir.
    1994).
    The only subject that requires extended discussion
    is Phillips’s contention that Ruby’s selection of his
    photo was the result of an unreliable process. Identifica-
    tion by a single eyewitness who lacks an apparent
    grudge against the accused person supplies probable
    cause for arrest. See, e.g., Gramenos v. Jewel Companies,
    Inc., 
    797 F.2d 432
    (7th Cir. 1986); Askew v. Chicago, 
    440 F.3d 894
    (7th Cir. 2006). But Phillips maintains that an
    identification should not count if it was the result of
    No. 10-3559                                                 5
    unduly suggestive procedures. He relies on decisions
    such as Neal v. Biggers, 
    409 U.S. 188
    (1972), which holds
    that a person’s selection of a suspect from a photo
    spread or lineup cannot be used as evidence at trial if
    the selection was the result of unduly suggestive proce-
    dures that create an excessive likelihood of irreparable
    misidentification. See also Simmons v. United States, 
    390 U.S. 377
    , 384 (1968).
    Biggers and similar decisions don’t support Phillips’s
    position directly. They concern the admissibility of evi-
    dence at criminal trials, not claims for damages against
    arresting officers. Similarly, decisions such as United
    States v. Johnson, 
    859 F.2d 1289
    , 1294 (7th Cir. 1988), and
    United States ex rel. Hudson v. Brierton, 
    699 F.2d 917
    , 923–24
    (7th Cir. 1983), whose significance the parties debate,
    concern the admissibility of evidence at a criminal trial.
    But Phillips contends that the Biggers approach should
    be extended from trials to arrests, and from a rule
    of evidence to a rule of damages. We think that the pro-
    posed extension would be improvident—though with
    an important proviso that we discuss later.
    The Supreme Court stressed in Gates that evidence
    need not be admissible at trial in order to support a
    finding of probable cause. A conclusion that there is
    probable cause for arrest (or indictment) just gets the
    criminal process started. Many later steps shape what
    evidence can be used at a trial and how much is
    required for conviction. Doctrines, such as that of
    Biggers, that are designed to reduce the chance of
    erroneous conviction do not have any greater function
    6                                                No. 10-3559
    at the arrest stage than do doctrines such as the require-
    ment of proof beyond a reasonable doubt, the de-
    fendant’s entitlement to confront his accusers, or the
    hearsay rule. Probable cause is established, and arrests
    are made, without an adversarial presentation. Applica-
    tion of the Biggers framework is possible, however,
    only after evidence has been gathered and an adversarial
    hearing held.
    Phillips wants such a proceeding held now and the
    results used to determine the validity of the arrest. But
    hindsight is not an appropriate basis for awarding dam-
    ages against police. The validity of an arrest depends
    on what is known at the moment of the arrest, not on
    evidence that may be developed years later. Phillips
    believes that he can get damages without the benefit of
    hindsight. He tells us that officer Allen must have
    known that he was steering Ruby to pick Phillips and
    that a reasonable officer must have known that priming
    a witness in this way would produce an inaccurate selec-
    tion. The first part of this argument is cut short by the
    rule that the officer’s motives do not count; probable
    cause is objective. See, e.g., Ashcroft v. al-Kidd, 
    131 S. Ct. 2074
    , 2080–83 (2011); Devenpeck v. Alford, 
    543 U.S. 146
    , 153
    (2004); Whren v. United States, 
    517 U.S. 806
    (1996). And
    the second part of Phillips’s argument fares no better.
    Whether mentioning a name and a rumor in a victim’s
    presence influences a later identification is a difficult
    question of psychology, not something about which
    police officers (and lawyers) know the right answer
    instinctively. Bufkin’s statements may have meant
    No. 10-3559                                               7
    nothing to Ruby. She says that she knew neither the
    name nor Phillips’s appearance; he contends, to the
    contrary, that she must have known his name and face
    because the two lived in the same neighborhood. In
    court, on a motion for summary judgment, we must
    assume that Phillips is right about this; but in a
    hospital room years ago, Allen was not bound to know
    that this was so. (Allen does not concede it even today,
    nor has Ruby ever admitted knowing plaintiff’s name
    and appearance before she selected his photograph.)
    Suppose Ruby is lying and did know Wydrick Phillips
    by name and appearance; would it follow that her knowl-
    edge of a tip that he had been robbing people would
    lead her to identify him as her assailant? Phillips’s
    lawyer treats an affirmative answer as obvious, but
    nothing is obvious about the psychology of eyewitness
    identification. Indeed, one point well established in the
    psychology literature is that most people’s intuitions on
    the subject of identification are wrong. See Christopher
    Chabris & Daniel Simons, The Invisible Gorilla: How Our
    Intuitions Deceive Us (2010). We held in United States v.
    Williams, 
    522 F.3d 809
    (7th Cir. 2008), that someone who
    contends that a particular kind of procedure led to an
    unreliable identification needs evidence—if not from an
    expert’s affidavit, then from published work such as
    Elizabeth F. Loftus, et al., Eyewitness Testimony: Civil and
    Criminal (4th ed. 2007), the standard text in this field.
    Phillips has not referred us to such evidence; he has only
    a lawyer’s confidence that what Allen did would have
    produced a worthless identification. Lawyers’ talk is no
    substitute for data.
    8                                               No. 10-3559
    Indeed, the approach of Biggers itself has been ques-
    tioned by social scientists. The American Psychological
    Association filed a brief as amicus curiae in Perry v. New
    Hampshire, No. 10-8974 (U.S. Jan. 11, 2012), in which
    the Court declined to extend Biggers to acts by private
    parties that may influence eyewitnesses. The Associa-
    tion told the Justices:
    In Biggers and Manson [v. Brathwaite, 
    432 U.S. 98
        (1977)], this Court enumerated five factors rele-
    vant to the probable accuracy of an eyewitness
    identification: “the opportunity of the witness to
    view the criminal at the time of the crime, the
    witness’ degree of attention, the accuracy of his
    prior description of the criminal, the level of
    certainty demonstrated at the confrontation,
    and the time between the crime and the confron-
    tation.” 
    Manson, 432 U.S. at 114
    (citing 
    Biggers, 409 U.S. at 199-200
    ). As shown by the discussion in
    the text, most of these factors are indeed
    relevant to probable accuracy—with the notable
    exception of witness certainty, see infra n.14.
    But given that notable exception, and given
    the plethora of other accuracy-related factors that
    researchers have identified since Biggers and
    Manson, APA urges the Court, in an appropriate
    case, to revisit the Manson framework so as to
    bring it in line with current scientific knowledge.
    APA Brief at 13 n.8. Footnote 14, to which note 8 refers,
    adds:
    No. 10-3559                                               9
    Jurors’ evident belief that eyewitness confidence
    correlates with accurate identifications was once
    shared by many in the judiciary. Indeed, in
    Biggers this Court stated, albeit without citing
    any scientific authorities, that confidence is an
    indication of accuracy. 
    See 409 U.S. at 199-200
    .
    Subsequent research, however, has called this
    notion into very serious question. As one report
    concluded, “[t]he outcomes of empirical studies,
    reviews, and meta-analyses have converged
    on the conclusion that the confidence-accuracy
    relationship for eyewitness identification is
    weak, with average confidence-accuracy correla-
    tions generally estimated between little more
    than 0 and .29.” Brewer et al., The Confidence-Accu-
    racy Relationship in Eyewitness Identification, 8 J.
    Experimental Psychol. Applied 44, 44-45 (2002).
    Even these various correlation figures are likely
    overestimates, moreover, because the confidence
    of eyewitnesses in actual cases, unlike in con-
    trolled experiments, may be infected by positive
    feedback received in the investigative process (for
    example, an officer stating during a photo array
    or line-up, “good, you identified the suspect”).
    [Citations omitted.]
    The Court bypassed this topic in Perry, leaving to the
    future any inquiry into the Biggers framework. Perry
    holds that in the main the validity of an eyewitness
    identification is for the jury—which implies that it is not
    tortious to obtain that identification in order to have
    evidence to present at trial. Neither the Supreme Court
    10                                              No. 10-3559
    nor this court has held that, as a matter of law, men-
    tioning a suspect’s name spoils an identification.
    Without a solid basis in the social science of eyewitness
    identification, a court could not appropriately create
    such a rule. Phillips has not offered any basis, solid
    or otherwise, for us to do so.
    We mentioned earlier the need for a proviso. It is this.
    Suppose a particular technique that officers may use
    to trick a person into making an unreliable identifica-
    tion has already been forbidden by an authoritative
    judicial decision (that is, by the Supreme Court or the
    court of appeals with territorial jurisdiction) and the
    officer uses it anyway. An officer who employs the for-
    bidden technique in order to manufacture an identifica-
    tion can’t complain when a court provides a remedy.
    Several courts of appeals have concluded that, when a
    state actor deliberately uses a forbidden technique to
    generate a false identification, an award of damages is
    permissible under 42 U.S.C. §1983. See, e.g., Good v. Curtis,
    
    601 F.3d 393
    , 398–99 (5th Cir. 2010) (intentionally manipu-
    lating a lineup); Brodnicki v. Omaha, 
    75 F.3d 1261
    , 1265–66
    (8th Cir. 1996) (dictum; officer held not liable). This
    approach does not assist Phillips, however, because no
    judicial decision has held that mentioning a suspect’s
    name in the hearing of a potential witness is improper.
    What Allen did was not remotely similar to showing
    a witness just one photo and asking her to “confirm”
    that the photo depicts the culprit.
    And if, despite the lack of scientific support, we were
    to create the sort of rule that Phillips proposes, it could
    No. 10-3559                                         11
    not do him any good. A rule of law devised after the
    events does not support an award of damages; officer
    Allen would be entitled to qualified immunity. See al-
    
    Kidd, 131 S. Ct. at 2083
    –85. It was not “clearly estab-
    lished” in February 2005, when Allen arrived in
    Elizabeth’s hospital room, that mentioning a suspect’s
    name in the presence of an eyewitness, and then
    showing that witness a photo spread containing the sus-
    pect’s picture, violates the Constitution.
    If Allen manipulated Ruby into selecting Phillips, he
    may have a remedy under state law. The district court
    dismissed all state-law claims without prejudice. But
    Phillips does not have a damages remedy for wrongful
    arrest under §1983 and the fourth amendment. None of
    the parties’ other arguments requires discussion.
    A FFIRMED
    2-10-12