United States v. Clacy Herrera , 704 F.3d 480 ( 2013 )


Menu:
  •                               In the
    United States Court of Appeals
    For the Seventh Circuit
    No. 11-2894
    U NITED S TATES OF A MERICA,
    Plaintiff-Appellee,
    v.
    C LACY W ATSON H ERRERA,
    Defendant-Appellant.
    Appeal from the United States District Court
    for the Northern District of Illinois, Eastern Division.
    No. 01 CR 1098-1—Rebecca R. Pallmeyer, Judge.
    A RGUED S EPTEMBER 28, 2012—D ECIDED JANUARY 9, 2013
    Before P OSNER, R OVNER, and SYKES, Circuit Judges.
    P OSNER, Circuit Judge. Two years ago, in response to
    a petition for a writ of mandamus filed by the govern-
    ment during the criminal trial of the defendant on drug
    charges, we ordered the district court to admit into evi-
    dence an exhibit labeled “Roberson Seizure 2”; to allow
    the government to recall Stephen Koop to testify at
    trial about the recovery of latent fingerprints from that
    exhibit; and to allow testimony regarding comparison of
    2                                                 No. 11-2894
    the latent prints with patent fingerprints known to be the
    defendant’s. In re United States, 
    614 F.3d 661
     (7th Cir.
    2010). The judge had excluded the exhibit and related
    testimony because he suspected, though on the most
    tenuous of grounds, that the government had tampered
    with the fingerprint evidence. He threatened to grant
    the defendant’s request for a mistrial on the ground of
    prosecutorial misconduct that was (the judge believed)
    intended to avert a likely acquittal, a ground that if sus-
    tained would have barred any further prosecution of the
    defendant as placing him in double jeopardy. Oregon v.
    Kennedy, 
    456 U.S. 667
    , 679 (1982); United States v. Catton, 
    130 F.3d 805
    , 807-08 (7th Cir. 1997); see also United States v.
    Buljubasic, 
    808 F.2d 1260
    , 1265 (7th Cir. 1987). We also
    ordered the case reassigned to another district judge.
    This was done and the trial, which had been interrupted
    by the mandamus proceeding, resumed, and ended
    shortly in the conviction of the defendant. The judge
    sentenced him to 340 months in prison for a variety of
    drug-related offenses. He appeals.
    Many of his arguments repeat ones he made in the
    mandamus proceeding. (In effect he is asking us to
    rehear our previous decision—two years after the dead-
    line for asking for rehearing expired.) The only such
    argument that we didn’t discuss is based on Will v.
    United States, 
    389 U.S. 90
    , 96-97 (1967), which forbids
    the use of mandamus as a substitute for an appeal that
    is forbidden—and the government is not permitted to
    appeal an evidentiary ruling in a criminal case once
    the trial has begun. 18 U.S.C. § 3731. But the Court in
    Will held only that the court of appeals hadn’t explained
    No. 11-2894                                               3
    why the district court’s ordering the government to
    give the defendant a bill of particulars was so
    “seriously disruptive of the efficient administration of
    criminal justice in the Northern District of Illinois” as to
    warrant mandamus. 389 U.S. at 104. The district judge’s
    order in the present case was no run-of-the-mill mistaken
    procedural or evidentiary ruling. The order seriously
    disrupted the prosecution’s case, and did so, as we
    are about to show, on the basis of utterly baseless but
    damaging imputations of grave (criminal, really) pros-
    ecutorial misconduct; involved the flouting of gov-
    erning precedents; and would probably have resulted
    in a groundless acquittal. The order thus warranted
    correction by mandamus. See United States v. Vinyard, 
    539 F.3d 589
    , 591-92 (7th Cir. 2008).
    The chain of events that culminated in the mandamus
    proceeding had begun with the district judge’s decision
    to exclude evidence that two of the defendant’s finger-
    prints had been recovered from a bag of heroin wrapped
    in tape and further encased in condoms and found in
    a drug courier’s rectum. The heroin had been removed
    from the bag and placed in an evidence bag and then
    both it and the packaging (the tape and condoms) had
    been placed in another evidence bag and it was this
    second exhibit that was at issue. The district judge’s
    ground for excluding it was his belief that the govern-
    ment hadn’t adequately demonstrated the requisite
    “chain of custody”—hadn’t demonstrated that there
    had been no opportunity to tamper with or otherwise
    mishandle the evidence between the time it was
    obtained and the trial. The judge made this ruling in the
    4                                              No. 11-2894
    face of the government’s having offered ten witnesses
    to establish that the chain of custody had remained intact.
    The judge was disturbed because the exhibit had,
    according to an evidence log sheet, gained 20 grams in
    weight between May and September 2001. (Yet he
    attached no significance to its having gained 190 grams
    between September 2001 and the trial.) He thought
    the weight gain might have been attributable to fed-
    eral officers’ pressing a piece of adhesive tape con-
    taining the defendant’s fingerprints (obtained else-
    where) onto the packaging of the heroin. That suspicion
    grew into a conviction, for which there was no rational
    basis, that government lawyers had lied about the chain
    of custody. To no avail the government explained that
    the reason for the increase in weight was that the bag
    with the fingerprints, after being opened so that the
    presence and amount of the illegal drug contained in
    it could be verified, and later closed up again, had been
    weighed together with other bags. The reported weight
    was the weight of the package containing all the
    bags, and thus there were more bags in it. Obviously
    the package would not have gained 210 grams (20 +
    190)—almost half a pound—from replacing a piece of the
    tape in which one of the bags was wrapped by a piece
    of tape containing the defendant’s fingerprints.
    The judge acknowledged that his supposition of tam-
    pering was “speculative.” That was an understatement.
    For among other things the defendant had not been
    extradited to the United States until long after the
    alleged tampering, and until he was extradited the gov-
    No. 11-2894                                                   5
    ernment did not have a set of fingerprints known to be
    his. And no one has explained how fingerprints on
    another piece of material could have been transferred
    to the adhesive side of the tape, which was where they
    were found. It’s one thing to press your finger on the
    adhesive side of a tape and remove the finger, leaving
    a print, but another thing to press a piece of paper con-
    taining your fingerprint on the adhesive side of
    the tape—try removing the paper without destroying
    the print.
    The defendant’s petition, and amended petition, for
    rehearing did not defend the judge’s conjecture that
    the weight discrepancy indicated tampering. We con-
    cluded that while the defendant could argue at trial
    that the jury should disregard the fingerprint evidence,
    there was no justification for excluding it in advance
    of trial on the “speculative” ground excogitated by
    the judge. Once the government presents evidence, as
    it did here (remember the ten witnesses), that adequate
    precautions had been taken to preserve the evidence
    challenged by the defendant, it has established admissi-
    bility, though at trial the defendant can challenge the
    adequacy of the precautions and present evidence of
    tampering. United States v. Lee, 
    502 F.3d 691
    , 697-98
    (7th Cir. 2007); United States v. Kelly, 
    14 F.3d 1169
    , 1175 (7th
    Cir. 1994); United States v. Brumfield, 
    686 F.3d 960
    , 965
    (8th Cir. 2012); see also Melendez-Diaz v. Massachusetts,
    
    557 U.S. 305
    , 311 n. 1 (2009). And that means by the
    way that even if our mandamus order was ultra vires
    it didn’t undermine the fairness of the trial or the justice
    of the defendant’s conviction. The fingerprint evidence
    6                                               No. 11-2894
    should not have been excluded, and once admitted con-
    firmed his guilt. We take up at the end of our opinion
    the defendant’s distinct argument that the reassignment
    of the case to another judge prejudiced the jury, and
    show that that argument has no merit either.
    The fresh issue relating to the fingerprint evidence
    is whether the prints of two fingers found on the
    adhesive tape were the defendant’s. They were latent
    rather than patent fingerprints. Patent fingerprints are
    made by pressing a fingertip covered with ink on a
    white card or similar white surface, and are visible.
    Latent fingerprints are prints, usually invisible, left on a
    smooth surface when a person touches it with a finger
    or fingers. Laboratory techniques are employed to make
    a latent fingerprint visible so that it can be compared
    with other fingerprints. The latent prints on the adhesive
    tape on the bag of heroin in this case were found by
    a fingerprint examiner to match the defendant’s patent
    prints made in the course of the criminal investigation,
    and the government therefore offered the match as evi-
    dence of the defendant’s participation in the drug ring.
    The defendant argues that methods of matching latent
    prints with other latent prints or with patent prints
    have not been shown to be reliable enough to be
    admissible as evidence under the standard for
    reliability set forth in Fed. R. Evid. 702, 703; Daubert v.
    Merrell Dow Pharmaceuticals, Inc., 
    509 U.S. 579
    , 592-93
    (1993); and Kumho Tire Co. v. Carmichael, 
    526 U.S. 137
    ,
    149 (1999).
    The method the examiner used is called ACE-V and
    is the standard method for determining whether two
    No. 11-2894                                                   7
    fingerprints are from the same person. See Scientific
    Working Group on Friction Ridge Analysis, Study and
    Technology, “Standards for Examining Friction Ridge
    Impressions and Resulting Conclusions,” Sept. 13, 2011,
    www.swgfast.org/documents/examinations-conclusions/
    111026_Examinations-Conclusions_1.0.pdf (visited Jan. 4,
    2013); Michele Triplett & Lauren Cooney, “The Etiology
    of ACE-V and Its Proper Use: An Exploration of the
    Relationship Between ACE-V and the Scientific Method
    of Hypothesis Testing,” 56 J. Forensic Identification 345, 346
    (2006). The defendant is therefore mounting a frontal
    assault on the use of fingerprint evidence in litigation, an
    attack the courts have frequently rebuffed. See, e.g.,
    United States v. Havvard, 
    260 F.3d 597
    , 601 (7th Cir. 2001);
    United States v. George, 
    363 F.3d 666
    , 672-73 (7th Cir.
    2004); United States v. Crisp, 
    324 F.3d 261
    , 268-70 (4th
    Cir. 2003); United States v. Mitchell, 
    365 F.3d 215
    , 235-
    46 (3d Cir. 2004).
    ACE-V is an acronym for analysis, comparison, evalua-
    tion, and verification, and has been described as follows:
    The process begins with the analysis of the unknown
    friction ridge print (now often a digital image of
    a latent print). Many factors affect the quality and
    quantity of detail in the latent print and also
    introduce variability in the resulting impression . . . . If
    the examiner deems that there is sufficient detail
    in the latent print (and the known prints), the com-
    parison of the latent print to the known prints begins.
    Visual comparison consists of discerning, visually
    “measuring,” and comparing—within the comparable
    areas of the latent print and the known prints—the
    8                                                No. 11-2894
    details that correspond. The amount of friction
    ridge detail available for this step depends on the
    clarity of the two impressions. The details observed
    might include the overall shape of the latent print,
    anatomical aspects, ridge flows, ridge counts, shape of
    the core, delta location and shape, lengths of the
    ridges, minutia location and type, thickness of the
    ridges and furrows, shapes of the ridges, pore
    position, crease patterns and shapes, scar shapes,
    and temporary feature shapes (e.g., a wart).
    At the completion of the comparison, the examiner
    performs an evaluation of the agreement of the
    friction ridge formations in the two prints and evalu-
    ates the sufficiency of the detail present to establish
    an identification (source determination). Source de-
    termination is made when the examiner concludes,
    based on his or her experience, that sufficient
    quantity and quality of friction ridge detail is in
    agreement between the latent print and the known
    print. Source exclusion is made when the process
    indicates sufficient disagreement between the latent
    print and known print. If neither an identification
    nor an exclusion can be reached, the result
    of the comparison is inconclusive. Verification occurs
    when another qualified examiner repeats the observa-
    tions and comes to the same conclusion, although
    the second examiner may be aware of the conclu-
    sion of the first.
    National Research Council of the National Academy of
    Sciences, Strengthening Forensic Science in the United States:
    A Path Forward 137-38 (2009).
    No. 11-2894                                                9
    The methodology requires recognizing and categorizing
    scores of distinctive features in the prints, see Davide
    Maltoni et al., Handbook of Fingerprint Recognition 97-101
    (2d ed. 2009); Federal Bureau of Investigation, The
    Science of Fingerprints: Classification and Uses 5-86 (2006),
    and it is the distinctiveness of these features, rather
    than the ACE-V method itself, that enables expert finger-
    print examiners to match fingerprints with a high
    degree of confidence. That’s not to say that fingerprint
    matching (especially when it involves latent finger-
    prints, as in this case) is as reliable as DNA evidence, for
    example. Forensic DNA analysis involves comparing a
    strand of DNA (the genetic code) from the suspect with
    a strand of DNA found at the crime scene. The
    comparison is done with scientific instruments and deter-
    mines whether the segments are chemically identical.
    Errors are vanishingly rare provided that the strands
    of code are reasonably intact. As we explained in
    United States v. Ford, 
    683 F.3d 761
    , 768 (7th Cir. 2012),
    What is involved, very simply, in forensic DNA
    analysis is comparing a strand of DNA (the genetic
    code) from the suspect with a strand of DNA found at
    the crime scene. See “DNA Profiling,” Wikipedia,
    http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/DNA_profiling (visited
    May 31, 2012). Comparisons are made at various
    locations on each strand. At each location there is
    an allele (a unique gene form). In one location, for
    example, the probability of a person’s having a par-
    ticular allele might be 7 percent, and in another
    10 percent. Suppose that the suspect’s DNA and the
    DNA at the crime scene contained the same alleles
    10                                              No. 11-2894
    at each of the two locations. The probability that
    the DNA was someone else’s would be 7 percent if the
    comparison were confined to the first location, but
    only .7 percent (7 percent of 10 percent) if the com-
    parison were expanded to two locations, because
    the probabilities are independent. Suppose identical
    alleles were found at 10 locations, which is what
    happened in this case; the probability that two
    persons would have so many identical alleles, a proba-
    bility that can be computed by multiplying together
    the probabilities of an identical allele at each
    location, becomes infinitesimally small—in fact 1 in
    29 trillion, provided no other comparisons reveal
    that the alleles at the same location on the two
    strands of DNA are different. This is the same proce-
    dure used for determining the probability that a
    perfectly balanced coin flipped 10 times in a row will
    come up heads all 10 times. The probability is .51 0 ,
    which is less than 1 in 1000.
    Chemical tests can determine whether two alleles are
    identical, but a fingerprint analyst must visually
    recognize and classify the relevant details in the latent
    print—which is difficult if the print is incomplete or
    smudged. “[T]he assessment of latent prints from crime
    scenes is based largely on human interpretation. . . . [T]he
    process does not allow one to stipulate specific measure-
    ments in advance, as is done for a DNA analysis. More-
    over, a small stretching of distance between two finger-
    print features, or a twisting of angles, can result from
    either a difference between the fingers that left the
    prints or from distortions from the impression process.”
    National Research Council, supra, at 139.
    No. 11-2894                                              11
    Matching latent fingerprints is thus a bit like an
    opinion offered by an art expert asked whether an un-
    signed painting was painted by the known painter of
    another painting; he makes or rejects a match on the
    basis of visual evidence. Eyewitness evidence is similar.
    The eyewitness saw the perpetrator of a crime. His recol-
    lection of the perpetrator’s appearance is analogous to
    a latent fingerprint. He sees the defendant at the
    trial—that sighting is analogous to a patent fingerprint.
    He is asked to match his recollection against the court-
    room sighting—and he is allowed to testify that the
    defendant is the perpetrator, not just that there is a close
    resemblance. A lineup, whether photo or in-person, is
    a related method of adducing matching evidence, as
    is handwriting evidence.
    Matching evidence of the kinds that we’ve just
    described, including fingerprint evidence, is less
    rigorous than the kind of scientific matching involved
    in DNA evidence; eyewitness evidence is not scientific
    at all. But no one thinks that only scientific evidence
    may be used to convict or acquit a defendant. The in-
    creasingly well documented fallibility of eyewitness
    testimony, see Elizabeth F. Loftus et al., Eyewitness Testi-
    mony: Civil and Criminal (4th ed. 2007); United States v.
    Ford, supra, 683 F.3d at 764-66, has not banished it from
    criminal trials. Perry v. New Hampshire, 
    132 S. Ct. 716
    ,
    728 (2012).
    Evidence doesn’t have to be infallible to be probative.
    Probability of guilt is a function of all the evidence in
    a case, and if items of evidence are independent of one
    12                                                No. 11-2894
    another in the sense that the truth of any one item is
    not influenced by the truth of any other, the probability
    of guilt may be much higher if there is evidence
    from many independent sources (several eyewitnesses,
    an eyewitness plus fingerprints, etc.) than it would be
    were there only the evidence of one eyewitness, say. If
    “the prosecution submits three items of evidence of the
    defendant’s guilt (and the defendant submits no
    evidence of his innocence), and the probability that item 1
    is spurious is 10 percent, the probability that item 2
    is spurious is also 10 percent, and likewise item 3 [,
    then the] probability that all three are spurious (assuming
    that the probabilities are independent—that is, that
    the probability that one piece of evidence is spurious
    does not affect the probability that another is), and there-
    fore that the defendant should be acquitted, is only one
    in a thousand (.1 x .1 x .1).” United States v. Williams,
    
    698 F.3d 374
    , 379 (7th Cir. 2012).
    The defendant intimates that any evidence that
    requires the sponsorship of an expert witness, as finger-
    print evidence does, must be found to be good science
    before it can be admitted under the doctrine of the
    Daubert case and Rules 702 or 703 of the Federal Rules
    of Evidence. But expert evidence is not limited to “scien-
    tific” evidence, however such evidence might be de-
    fined. Kumho Tire Co. v. Carmichael, supra, 526 U.S. at 150-51;
    Tuf Racing Products, Inc. v. American Suzuki Motor
    Corp., 
    223 F.3d 585
    , 591 (7th Cir. 2000). It includes any
    evidence created or validated by expert methods
    and presented by an expert witness that is shown to
    be reliable. In a case involving an alleged forgery of a
    No. 11-2894                                             13
    painting, there might be expert scientific evidence based
    on tests of the age of the canvas or paint; but there
    might also be expert evidence, offered by a dealer or art
    historian or other art expert, on the style of a particular
    artist. That evidence would be the expert’s opinion,
    based on comparison with other paintings, of the genuine-
    ness of the painting alleged to be a forgery. See, e.g.,
    Levin v. Dalva Brothers, Inc., 
    459 F.3d 68
    , 78-79 (1st Cir.
    2006); United States v. Tobin, 
    576 F.2d 687
    , 690-91, 693
    (5th Cir. 1978).
    Fingerprint experts such as the government’s witness
    in this case—who has been certified as a latent print
    examiner by the International Association for Identifica-
    tion, the foremost international fingerprint organiza-
    tion (there are only about 840 IAI-certified latent
    examiners in the world, out of 15,000 total examin-
    ers)—receive extensive training; and errors in finger-
    print matching by expert examiners appear to be
    very rare. Of the first 194 prisoners in the United States
    exonerated by DNA evidence, none had been convicted
    on the basis of erroneous fingerprint matches, whereas
    75 percent had been convicted on the basis of mistaken
    eyewitness identification. Greg Hampikian et al.,
    “The Genetics of Innocence: Analysis of 194 U.S. DNA
    Exonerations,” 12 Annual Rev. of Genomics and Human
    Genetics 97, 106 (2011). The probability of two people in
    the world having identical fingerprints is not known,
    but it appears to be extremely low. Steven M. Stigler,
    “Galton and Identification by Fingerprints,” 140 Genetics
    857, 858 (1995); David A. Stoney & John I. Thornton,
    14                                              No. 11-2894
    “A Critical Analysis of Quantitative Fingerprint Indi-
    viduality Models,” 31 J. of Forensic Sciences 1187 (1986).
    The great statistician Francis Galton estimated the proba-
    bility as 1 in 64 billion. Galton, Finger Prints 110 (1892);
    Stigler, supra at 858. That was not an estimate of the
    probability of a mistaken matching of a latent to a
    patent or another latent fingerprint. Yet errors in such
    matching appear to be very rare, though the matching
    process is judgmental rather than scientifically rigorous
    because it depends on how readable the latent finger-
    print is and also on how distorted a version of the
    person’s patent fingerprint it is. Examiners’ training
    includes instruction on how to determine whether a
    latent print contains enough detail to enable a reliable
    matching to another print. Ultimately the matching
    depends on “subjective judgments by the examiner,”
    National Research Council, supra, at 139, but responsible
    fingerprint matching is admissible evidence, in general
    and in this case.
    The other issues presented by the appeal that merit
    discussion arise from the interruption of the trial by the
    mandamus proceeding and the resulting reassignment
    of the case to a different district judge. The con-
    sequence was an eleven-day hiatus in the trial. The defen-
    dant argues that when the trial resumed, the jurors,
    remembering the skeptical remarks that the original
    judge had made about the government’s evidence, must
    have thought that he had been “punished” for siding
    with the defendant by being removed and therefore
    that the jury should convict. That is unpersuasive con-
    No. 11-2894                                            15
    jecture. Because of sickness most commonly, but some-
    times for other reasons, such as belated discovery of
    a ground for recusal, a judge is sometimes replaced
    during a trial and when that happens the new judge
    tells the jury that such replacements happen oc-
    casionally and the jurors are not to worry about the
    change in judges or speculate about the reason for it.
    The new judge in this case didn’t explain the cause of
    the delays but did say:
    It is very important for me to emphasize this instruc-
    tion, that however you may feel about the delays
    in this case, you are not to hold those feelings
    against anybody in this courtroom . . . . In fact, I am
    going to instruct you right now that you not
    speculate about the causes or reasons for the delays
    at all . . . . To the extent that you have been told or
    you have come to believe that the delays are
    somehow the fault of the government or the fault of
    the defense counsel, I am instructing you that you
    put those concerns out of your mind completely . . . .
    At the end of this case, we will not be asking you, did
    the trial go smoothly? And if not, whose fault was it?
    That will not be a question you will be asked to con-
    sider. The only question you will be asked to consider
    at the conclusion of this case is, did the government
    meet its burden of proof? That’s the only question.
    And concerns about delays are not to be in your
    mind at all . . . . From time to time there are
    reasons that we have to interrupt the smooth progress
    of a trial. It’s happened to me before. This was one of
    those occasions . . . . Your consideration of the
    16                                               No. 11-2894
    evidence should not be influenced in any way by
    any assumptions you may have made or any conclu-
    sions you may have drawn about delays.
    There is no history of which we’re aware of
    miscarriages of justice resulting because juries draw
    erroneous inferences from the replacement of a judge.
    See United States v. Gayles, 
    1 F.3d 735
    , 738 (8th Cir. 1993);
    United States v. LaSorsa, 
    480 F.2d 522
    , 531 (2d Cir. 1973).
    The defendant complains that the new judge pres-
    sured the jury to complete its deliberations in a day
    and that with more time it might have acquitted him.
    There is no evidence to support that accusation of a
    judge noted for her patience. The first judge had assured
    the jury that the trial would not interfere with any of
    the jurors’ vacation schedules. When trial resumed on
    August 2 the jury was down to 12 because one of the
    two alternates had been excused and the other had re-
    placed a juror who had been excused. One of the
    remaining jurors had long-standing vacation plans for
    August 5, and the original judge had (with the govern-
    ment’s consent) assured her when the government
    sought mandamus and the trial was adjourned that
    she would not need to show up on or after that date.
    When the trial resumed, another juror asked in open
    court what the jury should do in light of the possibility
    that the juror with vacation plans would leave before
    the trial ended. In response, and without objection by
    the defendant’s lawyer, the judge said “we can’t proceed”
    with fewer than 12 jurors. That was true (since the
    parties would not stipulate to a jury of 11, see Fed. R. Crim.
    No. 11-2894                                                17
    P. 23(b)(2)), though what was also true but she rightly
    did not say, because it would have sown confusion, is
    that while the trial could not continue without 12 jurors,
    if once the jury retired for its deliberations one of the
    jurors then decamped the judge could allow the
    remaining 11 to render a verdict even without the law-
    yers’ consent. Fed. R. Crim. P. 23(b)(3).
    August 4 turned out to be the last day of the trial.
    Closing arguments and the reading of the instructions
    to the jury took until the afternoon. The jury retired to
    consider its verdict at about 3:45 and returned 7 hours
    later with a verdict of guilty on eight counts and not
    guilty on the remaining six. The defendant argues that
    the jurors had rushed to complete their deliberations,
    knowing there would not be 12 jurors the next day.
    Given the strength of the government’s case and the
    length of the jury’s deliberations, and the fact that
    there was only one defendant and that the jury
    acquitted him on some counts, it is unlikely that even
    if they hadn’t been expecting to lose the twelfth juror
    the next day, the jurors would have taken more time
    to deliberate than they did, though they might have
    broken at dinner time and resumed the following morn-
    ing. The judge did not, as in the cases that the defendant
    cites to us, United States v. Blitch, 
    622 F.3d 658
    , 670 (7th
    Cir. 2010), and United States v. Chaney, 
    559 F.2d 1094
    , 1098
    (7th Cir. 1977), set a deadline, either explicit or implicit,
    for the jury’s deliberations. On the contrary, after instruct-
    ing the jury, and only moments before the jury left
    the courtroom to deliberate, the judge told them:
    “I think I mentioned earlier that from this point on, the
    18                                              No. 11-2894
    schedule is up to you. I realize [by] the way that the trial
    has been bumpy, and I will make every effort to accom-
    modate your schedule from this moment on, whatever
    your decisions are. I appreciate your time. I think all of
    us do. You are excused to deliberate on your verdict.”
    That was the opposite of pressuring the jury to complete
    its deliberations in a day. The jurors were unlikely to
    feel rushed when the judge had gone out of her way to
    tell them that she would make every effort to accom-
    modate their schedules. Had the jurors been unable to
    agree on a verdict on August 4, the foreman would have
    told the judge that they couldn’t reach a verdict and she
    would have either discharged them and declared a
    mistrial or allowed the 11 remaining jurors to return
    the next day and deliberate.
    When the jury retired to deliberate, knowing that one
    juror would leave on vacation the next day and perhaps
    believing that 12 jurors had to be present to render a
    verdict, no juror asked the judge a question such as:
    “Does this mean we must render a verdict by the end of
    the day or can we just report our inability to reach
    a verdict?” Or: “What if we can’t complete our delibera-
    tions by the end of the day?” Such questions would
    have flagged concerns that the judge would doubtless
    have addressed. No questions were asked. That
    suggests that the jurors were not concerned that the
    trial might end without a verdict unless they rushed
    their deliberations.
    A FFIRMED.
    1-9-13