In re Elias V. , 237 Cal. App. 4th 568 ( 2015 )


Menu:
  • Filed 6/9/15
    CERTIFIED FOR PUBLICATION
    IN THE COURT OF APPEAL OF THE STATE OF CALIFORNIA
    FIRST APPELLATE DISTRICT
    DIVISION TWO
    In re Elias V., a Person Coming Under the
    Juvenile Court Law.
    THE PEOPLE,
    Plaintiff and Respondent,
    v.                                                    A140263
    Elias V.,                                             (Sonoma County
    Defendant and Appellant.                      Super. Ct. No. 37612J)
    In an original wardship petition (Welf. & Inst. Code, § 602), appellant, Elias V.,
    then 13 years of age, was alleged to have committed a lewd and lascivious act upon a
    child under the age of 14 years. (Pen. Code, § 288, subd. (a).)1 Prior to and again at the
    time of the jurisdictional hearing, defense counsel moved to exclude inculpatory
    statements appellant made to the police on the ground they were involuntary and
    therefore inadmissible under Miranda v. Arizona (1966) 
    384 U.S. 436
    (Miranda). After a
    three-day hearing, the motion was denied and the petition sustained. Elias was declared a
    ward of the court and placed on probation in the home of his parents.
    Elias claims his confession was involuntary under the due process clause of the
    Fourteenth Amendment, as it was the product of the type of coercive interrogation
    techniques condemned in Miranda, which had “overborne his will.” Contending the
    statements were erroneously received in evidence and cannot be considered harmless, he
    maintains the judgment must be reversed. We agree.
    1
    All statutory references are to the Penal Code unless otherwise indicated.
    1
    FACTS
    Elias and his friend Hector T. lived across the hall from one another in a four-unit
    apartment building in Santa Rosa. The boys usually played together at Elias’s apartment,
    but on October 6, 2012, they played at Hector’s apartment in a bedroom shared by
    Hector, his mother, father, brother, and sister A.T., who was then 3 years old. Hector,
    nine years old at the time of the hearing, testified that while he and Elias were playing a
    video game, Elias lying on the bed and Hector sitting on the floor, A.T. climbed on the
    bed and lay down near Elias. Hector could not see the top of the bed from where he was
    sitting and did not hear anything Elias was saying. A.T. was laughing, the bedroom door
    was open, and Hector’s mother, Aurora, was in another room. The boys continued
    playing their video game until Aurora entered the room. Hector testified that when she
    came in, Elias was sitting on the bed. Aurora testified that when she entered the room
    she saw Elias lying on the edge of the bed next to A.T. and noticed that when A.T. got up
    her pants were at the middle of her leg. When Aurora asked, “what happened?” Elias
    said A.T. asked him to help take off her pants because “she wanted to go to the
    bathroom.” Aurora described Elias as “surprised” and “scared.”
    Aurora never testified that she saw Elias improperly touch A.T. on October 6th or
    at any other time. She believed he did only because the child was “talking about it all the
    time,” telling her and others, “ ‘This boy, he touched me.’ He did this he did that, you
    know, just in my head.” A.T. was interviewed on February 1, 2013, at the Redwood
    Children’s Center (RCC) by a person trained in interviewing very young children, and the
    interview was recorded, but the recording was never offered in evidence and the
    interviewer did not testify. The detective assigned the case, Mechelle Buchignani,
    observed the interview but was not asked about it by the prosecutor. However, on cross
    examination, she stated that A.T. “did say that he touched her in the RCC interview,” 2
    2
    Detective Buchignani also testified, more definitively, that the child was “very
    clear” about being touched by Elias “[i]n what she told her parents.” Buchignani
    testified that the RCC interview lasted only 10 minutes and was a “fairly typical 3-year-
    old interview.” Asked if she recalled that A.T. “doesn’t know how to count, she doesn’t
    know her colors, she doesn’t know where her head is,” Buchignani responded, “I believe
    she knows where her head is. She didn’t follow directions with moving the pen.”
    2
    and that she “showed us where he touched her” by pointing to the vaginal area on a doll.
    Asked, “[so she] doesn’t point to the stomach,” the detective responded, “[l]ooked like
    the vaginal area to me.”
    Aurora did not contact the police until October 23, 17 days after the incident, and
    her delay in reporting it became an issue at the jurisdictional hearing. The defense
    maintained that Aurora concocted the charge against Elias and contacted the police
    because she had just learned that the landlord intended to evict her family and falsely
    believed Elias’s father had put the landlord up to this.
    The landlord testified that starting before 2011, when Elias’s family moved in, she
    frequently spoke with Aurora and her husband Carlos about complaints from tenants on
    both floors of the building and from neighbors that people living in or visiting Aurora’s
    apartment (including Aurora’s husband and children, her brothers, and others) were
    playing loud music, playing volleyball and “drinking alcohol a lot” in the backyard,
    obstructing the carport, and “peeing” in the yard and in the laundry room. The landlord
    repeatedly told Aurora “ ‘please, you need to stop the drinking, the loud music. It just
    needs to stop.’ And, like I said, I would go there at least once a week just telling them,
    ‘Hey, you need to cut it out.’ ”
    The landlord finally realized “the complaints had gotten out of hand” when she
    learned from Elias’s father that Aurora’s brother, “wanted to take a swing” at him.
    Elias’s father “was scared, and you could hear it in his voice.” She evicted the family
    because she was “sick and tired” of the problems; the incident with Elias’s father “put the
    topping on the cake.” The landlord never told Aurora that Elias’s family asked her to
    evict her family, and denied that the eviction, which took place in November 2013, was
    retaliation for Aurora’s accusations against Elias. The landlord warned Aurora about
    eviction on October 22, the day after she heard about the incident with Elias’s father, and
    had also warned them previously that their behavior could lead to eviction, although at
    Buchignani agreed that A.T. was “off into some other subject matter” when asked
    questions. Asked whether the interview was so short in part because A.T. was not giving
    information that would be helpful, Buchignani stated, “She gave us enough information
    to believe she had been touched.”
    3
    another point she testified that the first time she discussed eviction with any member of
    Aurora’s household was in October 2012.
    Aurora complained to the police on October 23, the day after the landlord told her
    that her family was going to be evicted, but she denied that this had anything to do with
    Elias’s father complaining to the landlord about her family’s conduct. Her testimony on
    this issue, however, was inconsistent and confusing. According to Aurora, after she
    reported the incident with Elias and A.T., the landlord told Aurora’s sister-in-law that she
    was going to evict Aurora because of this report. Asked whether the landlord came to
    talk to her on October 20, shortly before she reported the incident to the police, Aurora
    responded that the landlord came that day to pick up the rent, and she asked her son
    Hector to “interpret for me and to tell her . . . [¶] [w]hat the boy, [Elias], had done to my
    daughter.” Aurora testified that the landlord did not say she was going to evict Aurora,
    only that she “wanted proof” of what [Elias] had done to A.T. Asked several times
    whether she told the landlord that her family could not be evicted “because [Elias] had
    touched [her] daughter,” Aurora refused to give a responsive answer until directed to do
    so by the court, after which she stated “No.” Aurora then testified that she did not
    remember any conversation with the landlord in October 2012: “I don’t remember
    having spoken to [the landlord] before she gave me the eviction notice” on November
    23.3 And, further confusing matters, Aurora testified that she met with the landlord on
    October 9, three days after the incident, and told her “what this boy has done to my
    daughter.” In response to the question why she had waited 17 days to report Elias’s
    conduct to the police, Aurora said she didn’t go immediately because the landlord said
    “she wanted proof. So then I said, ‘Okay. I’m going to file a report so that I can give
    you proof.’ She has—my daughter has to say what happened to her.” (Italics added.)
    3
    Aurora testified that before the eviction notice on November 23, the landlord
    had never told her of complaints from other tenants or warned her about loud music or
    fights near her apartment. Aurora acknowledged that prior to October 6, the landlord had
    threatened to evict her because of males from her apartment urinating in public in front of
    the apartment, but stated that she had not seen “anything like that,” her family members
    denied it happening, and the neighbors the landlord said had complained about it denied
    having done so.
    4
    Aurora said she did not go to the police immediately after the incident because “I
    didn’t want to get to a point where the problem gets the way that it’s turned out right
    now. That is why I spoke to the landlord to tell her what kind of people she had there.”
    Asked why she waited another two weeks to contact the police, Aurora said, “I did not
    know what to do. My daughter . . . was always talking about the same things, saying
    ‘This boy, he touched me.’ He did this he did that, you know . . . so I thought if I leave
    things the way they are, then my daughter’s going to end up being raped.” On October
    23, a friend told her to go to the police and she did so.
    After Aurora phoned the police on October 23, 2013, Sonoma County Deputy
    Sheriff Carlos Chavez was instructed to look into the matter and make an “incident
    report” that would be used to decide whether a detective should be assigned to investigate
    the case. On October 24, Chavez met with Aurora, who told him she believed Elias “had
    assaulted her daughter” on October 6th. Chavez did not interview anyone other than
    Aurora before submitting his incident report to the detectives for further investigation.
    Detective Buchignani was assigned to the case on October 24.
    About a month after Officer Chavez took Aurora’s statement regarding Elias, he
    was “dispatched” to look into a “complaint between neighbors” at the apartment house
    where Elias and Aurora lived. The complainant, Elias’s father, was concerned “that his
    next-door neighbors were rowdy, were constantly drinking downstairs in a picnic area,
    urinating on the fence, and there may have been a challenge to fight him, and him stating
    he didn’t want any problems.” Officer Chavez spoke with Aurora’s husband, related
    Elias’s father’s concerns and told Elias’s father and Aurora’s husband “to stay away from
    each other and be peaceful,” and “that was the end of the investigation.”
    Detective Buchignani interrogated Elias on February 6, 2013, more than three
    months after the case was assigned to her. At the time of the interrogation,4 Buchignani’s
    knowledge of the case was apparently based solely on Aurora’s statement to Officer
    Chavez, which was in turn based entirely on Chavez’s brief interview of Aurora, and
    4
    As we later explain (see especially discussion, post, at pp. 34-35), an
    “interrogation” is significantly different from an “interview.” Interrogation is an
    accusatory process involving active persuasion. An interview is a non-accusatory
    investigative tool designed to gather information and normally precedes an interrogation.
    5
    Buchignani’s observation of the 10-minute interview of A.T.5 Buchignani stated that at
    the time she interrogated Elias the Sheriff’s Office had not “[made] contact with anyone
    other than Aurora.” Neither Buchignani, nor, so far as she knew, any other officer, asked
    residents of the apartment complex about Elias’s behavior or whether “other children had
    been disturbed by Elias or anyone else.”
    On the day of the interrogation, Detective Buchignani, her partner Sergeant Ruben
    Martinez, and another unidentified deputy sheriff went to the elementary school to speak
    with Elias. The principal brought them to a small office used by a school counselor.
    When the principal returned to the room with Elias, Buchignani introduced herself and
    Sergeant Martinez, told Elias she had to tell him his legal rights, and then gave him the
    admonitions required by Miranda. Asked whether he ever previously had any contact
    with the police, Elias answered “no.”
    The subsequent interrogation, which we later relate in greater detail, consumed 20
    to 30 minutes. Throughout the session, Buchignani stated as a fact that Elias had touched
    A.T. in a sexual manner and needed help for his problem of attraction to a young child.
    For the vast majority of the interrogation, Elias adamantly denied Buchignani’s repeated
    assertions that he had touched A.T. in an improper manner; he portrayed A.T. as a
    somewhat annoying, very young child clamoring for his attention. and repeatedly
    explained that he had simply unzipped the child’s pants at her request. Finally, when
    Buchignani suggested Elias might have touched A.T.’s vagina because he found it
    exciting or just because he was curious, Elias rejected the first suggestion and, to
    Buchignani’s comment, “[b]ut you did it,” said, “[f]or curiosity.” Elias thus accepted
    Buchignani’s alternative theory that he touched the bare skin of A.T.’s vagina for three to
    four seconds, in the midst of playing a video game with her brother, merely “out of
    curiosity.”
    5
    Buchignani testified that she believed Elias’s denials in the interrogation were
    lies because “the child was very clear about being touched by him . . . [i]n what she told
    her parents.” Asked what Aurora told her that made her believe Elias touched A.T.,
    Buchignani replied, “What the child told the mother per the [incident] report.”
    Buchignani met with Aurora on January 1, 2013, to discuss the RCC interview and “see
    if they wanted to move forward with the case” but did not mention the contents of their
    discussion in her testimony.
    6
    Before and again at the jurisdictional hearing, defense counsel moved to exclude
    the statements on the grounds they were involuntary. On August 22, 2013, after the court
    took the case under submission, the court impliedly denied the motion and ruled that the
    charged offense had been committed.
    In finding Elias’s statements voluntary and reliable, the court observed that
    Detective Buchignani’s manner was “gentle” and “calm,” her questions “were short,” not
    “convoluted,” “the questions weren’t split where there would be two responses you’d
    have to use to the same single question,” and “her language usage for someone of Elias’s
    age was appropriate.” The court summed up its view this way: ”Just the totality of
    where the interview took place was, in the court’s view, not intimidating. It was very
    short. It was only a 20-minute interview. And it complied with the current case law. I
    don’t have a problem with the way the interview was conducted.” The court noted that
    “Elias was able to indicate in the flow of conversation [with Detective Buchignani] if he
    needed clarification of anything, and he did that a couple of times, and there was give and
    take in the conversation. In other words, sometimes he asked questions too, and that’s
    what really made me feel that this interview was appropriate.” The court thus impliedly
    concluded that Elias’s statements were “the product of his free and rational choice.”
    (Greenwald v. Wisconsin (1968) 
    390 U.S. 519
    , 521.)
    After the jurisdictional ruling, the Sonoma County Juvenile Probation Department
    interviewed Elias and his parents. Its report to the court states that Elias, who had never
    previously been charged with any delinquency, “indicated he only tried to help the young
    girl. The minor stated [that] prior to this incident, he liked helping other people and
    learned this trait from his father. As a result of this incident, he no longer wants to help
    others, as he feels his actions could be taken the wrong way. The minor maintains he did
    not commit this offense, was at her home playing video games with the victim’s younger
    brother, and the only thing he did was to try to help her.” Elias’s parents “maintain their
    son’s innocence, and stated the only thing their son did was to try to help the girl. They
    report the allegations stem from a dispute with the victim’s family because they blame
    the minor’s father for being evicted.” Elias’s mother said the charges against Elias have
    “caused a lot of conflict within the family as she told the minor’s father if he had not said
    7
    anything to the landlord this situation would never have occurred.” Elias’s parents also
    told probation officers that “Elias grew very frustrated over these proceedings and kept
    telling them he would admit to anything just to get this over with. However, they kept
    telling their son not to admit to something he did not do just because he was tired and
    frustrated. They believe this outcome is not ‘just,’ and they will continue their pursuit of
    the truth.”
    At the close of the dispositional hearing the court declared Elias a ward of the
    court and placed him on probation at the home of his parents, with numerous conditions.
    Notice of this appeal, which is authorized by Welfare and Institutions Code
    section 800, was timely filed on November 7, 2013.
    DISCUSSION
    I.
    The chief issue presented in this appeal is the voluntariness of the admissions Elias
    made at the close of Detective Buchignani’s interrogation. The use of an involuntary
    confession for any purpose in a criminal or delinquency proceeding violates a defendant’s
    or minor’s rights under the Fourteenth Amendment. (Arizona v. Fulminante (1991) 
    499 U.S. 279
    .)
    “The admissibility of a confession depends upon the totality of the circumstances
    existing at the time the confession was obtained. (People v. Robertson (1982) 
    33 Cal. 3d 21
    , 39-40; People v. Sanchez (1969) 
    70 Cal. 2d 562
    , 572, cert. dism., Sanchez v.
    California (1969) 
    394 U.S. 1025
    .) A minor can effectively waive his constitutional rights
    (People v. Lara (1967) 
    67 Cal. 2d 365
    , 390-391, cert. den. Lara v. California (1968) 
    392 U.S. 945
    . . . [fn. omitted] but age, intelligence, education and ability to comprehend the
    meaning and effect of his confession are factors in that totality of circumstances to be
    weighed along with other circumstances in determining whether the confession was a
    product of free will and an intelligent waiver of the minor’s Fifth Amendment rights
    ([Lara], at pp. 385-387).” (People v. Maestas (1987) 
    194 Cal. App. 3d 1499
    , 1508.)
    The federal and state Constitutions both require the prosecution to show the
    voluntariness of a confession by a preponderance of the evidence. (Lego v. Twomey
    (1972) 
    404 U.S. 477
    . 489; People v. Markham (1989) 
    49 Cal. 3d 63
    , 71.) Voluntariness
    8
    turns on all the surrounding circumstances, “both the characteristics of the accused and
    the details of the interrogation” (Schneckloth v. Bustamante (1973) 
    412 U.S. 218
    , 226); it
    does not depend on whether the confession is trustworthy. (Rogers v. Richmond (1961)
    
    365 U.S. 534
    , 543-544.) While a determination that a confession was involuntary
    requires a finding of coercive police conduct (Colorado v. Connelly (1986) 
    479 U.S. 157
    ;
    People v, Maury (2003) 
    30 Cal. 4th 342
    , 404), “ ‘ “the exertion of any improper
    influence” ’ ” by the police suffices. (Hutto v. Ross (1976) 
    429 U.S. 28
    , 30.)
    The issue of voluntariness presents “ ‘a mixed question of law and fact that is
    nevertheless predominantly legal . . . .’ [Citation.] Hence ‘ “[o]n appeal, the
    determination of a trial court as to the ultimate issue of voluntariness of a confession is
    reviewed independently . . . . [¶] The trial court’s determination concerning whether
    coercive police activity was present, whether certain conduct constituted a promise and, if
    so, whether it operated as an inducement, are apparently subject to independent review as
    well.” [Citation.] However, “the trial court’s findings as to the circumstances
    surrounding the confession—including ‘the characteristics of the accused and the details
    of the interrogation’ [citation]—are clearly subject to review for substantial
    evidence. . . .” ’ [Citation.]” (People v. Jones (1998) 
    17 Cal. 4th 279
    , 296.)
    II.
    As appellant asserts the techniques employed by Detective Buchignani to
    overcome his will were condemned by the Supreme Court in 
    Miranda, supra
    , 
    384 U.S. 436
    , we commence our analysis by discussing the portions of that opinion describing
    such techniques.
    The foundational theses of Miranda are that “the modern practice of in-custody
    interrogation is psychologically rather than physically oriented” (
    Miranda, supra
    , 384
    U.S. at p. 448), and the psychological techniques now employed by interrogators “trade[]
    on the weakness of individuals,” and “may even give rise to a false confession.” (Id. at
    p. 455, & fn. 24, citing Borchard, Convicting the Innocent (1932).)
    The danger of false confessions is real. Studies conducted after Miranda was
    decided estimate that between 42 and 55 percent of suspects confess in response to a
    custodial interrogation. (Kassin & Gudjonsson, The Psychology of Confessions: A
    9
    Review of the Literature and Issues, 5 Psych Sci. in the Public Interest 33, 44.)6
    Estimates of false confessions as the leading cause of error in wrongful convictions range
    from 14 to 25 percent, and as will be discussed (see, post, at pp. 21-25), a
    disproportionate number of false confession cases involve juveniles. Recent research has
    shown that more than one-third (35 percent) of proven false confessions were obtained
    from suspects under the age of 18. (Drizin & Leo, The Problem of False Confessions in
    the Post-DNA World (2004) 82 N.C.L.Rev. 891, 902, 944-945, fn. 5 (False
    Confessions).)
    Since Miranda, the Supreme Court has continued to express concern about false
    confessions. In Corley v. United States (2009) 
    556 U.S. 303
    , 320-321, the court observed
    again that “ ‘[c]ustodial police interrogation, by its very nature, isolates and pressures the
    individual,’ [citation] and there is mounting empirical evidence that these pressures can
    induce a frighteningly high percentage of people to confess to crimes they never
    committed.” (Ibid. ) Even more recently, the court indicated that its long-standing
    concern about false confessions may be most acute in cases involving the police
    interrogation of juveniles, particularly adolescents. (J.D.B. v. North Carolina (2012)
    ___U.S.___ [
    131 S. Ct. 2394
    , 2401] (J.D.B.).) An extensive body of literature
    demonstrates that juveniles are “more suggestible than adults, may easily be influenced
    by questioning from authority figures, and may provide inaccurate reports when
    questioned in a leading, repeated and suggestive fashion” (Meyer & Reppucci, Police
    Practices and Perceptions Regarding Juvenile Interrogation and Interrogative
    Suggestibility (2007) 25 Behavioral Sciences & the Law 757, 763; see also, Ceci &
    Bruck, Suggestibility of The Child Witness: A Historical Review and Synthesis (1993)
    113(3) Psychological Bulletin 403-409; Dunn, Questioning the Reliability of Children’s
    Testimony: An Examination of the Problematic Elements (1995) 19 Law & Psych. Rev.
    203-215; Owen-Kostelnick et al., Testimony and Interrogation of Minors: Assumptions
    About Maturity and Morality (2006) 61(4) American Psychologist 286-304), and that
    6
    The growing body of research evaluating the manner in which interrogative
    practices influence suspects and lead them to confess has profited from the increasing
    practice of recording custodial interrogations. (Ofshe & Leo, The Decision to Confess
    Falsely (1997) 74 Denver U. L.Rev. 979, 981-982 (Decision to Confess Falsely).)
    10
    “juveniles aged fifteen and younger have deficits in their legal understanding, knowledge,
    and decision-making capabilities.” (Redlich, The Susceptibility of Juveniles to False
    Confessions and False Guilty Pleas (2010) 62 Rutgers L.Rev. 943, 952 (Susceptibility of
    Juveniles); see Viljoen et al., Legal Decisions of Preadolescent and Adolescent
    Defendants: Predictors of Confessions, Pleas, Communication with Attorneys, and
    Appeals (2005) 29(3) Law & Human Behavior 253; Overbeck, No Match for the Police:
    An analysis of Miranda’s Problematic Application to Juvenile Defendants (2011) 38
    Hastings Const. L.Q. 1053, 1066-1069 (No Match).)
    Noting that interrogation at that time largely took place in private, and “privacy
    results in secrecy” which “in turn results in a gap in our knowledge of what in fact goes
    on in the interrogation rooms,” the Miranda court relied heavily on police manuals and
    texts relating to interrogation. (
    Miranda, supra
    , 384 U.S. at p. 448.) As these texts
    recommend tactics that have been effectively employed to obtain confessions, they “are
    used by law enforcement agencies themselves as guides . . . [and] professedly present the
    most enlightened and effective means presently used to obtain statements through
    custodial interrogation.” (Id. at p. 449.) These techniques, and indeed updated versions
    of many of the same texts, continue to be widely used today.
    John E. Reid & Associates was the largest national provider of training in
    interrogation techniques at the time Miranda was decided, and still is today. The basic
    course on “The Reid Technique” is predicated on the methodology first set forth in the
    initial edition of Inbau & Reid, Criminal Confessions and Interrogations (1962), a classic
    work which was quoted extensively by Chief Justice Warren in Miranda. The current
    Reid training manual, which remains the leading law enforcement treatise on custodial
    interrogation, was published in 2013. (Inbau et al., Criminal Interrogation and
    Confessions (5th ed. 2013) (hereafter Criminal Interrogation).) It has been estimated that
    about two-thirds of police executives in this nation have had training in the “Reid
    Method.” (Zalman & Smith, The Attitudes of Police Executives Towards Miranda and
    Interrogation Policies (2007) 97 J. Crim. L. & Criminology 873, 920.) 7
    7
    In California, local law enforcement agencies officers who may not have
    attended a Reid program receive training from academies whose curricula, mandated and
    11
    As will be seen, many of the techniques used to interrogate Elias derive from the
    Reid methodology described in Miranda. Behavioral scientists who study interrogation
    techniques and their effects have concisely described the Reid Technique as follows:
    “First, investigators are advised to isolate the suspect in a small private room, which
    increases his or her anxiety and incentive to escape. A nine-step process then ensues in
    which an interrogator employs both negative and positive incentives. On one hand, the
    interrogator confronts the suspect with accusations of guilt, assertions that may be
    bolstered by evidence, real or manufactured, and refuses to accept alibi’s and denials. On
    the other hand, the interrogator offers sympathy and moral justification, introducing
    ‘themes’ that minimize the crime and lead suspects to see confession as an expedient
    means of escape.” (Kassin et al., Police-Induced Confessions: Risk Factors and
    Recommendations (2010) 34 Law & Human Behav. 3, 7 (Police-Induced Confessions).)8
    According to these authors, the purpose of interrogation is “not to discern the truth,
    determine if the suspect committed the crime, or evaluate his or her denials. Rather,
    police are trained to interrogate only those suspects whose culpability they ‘establish’ on
    the basis of their initial investigation.” (Id. at p. 6.)
    approved by the Commission on Peace Officer Standards and Training (POST) (Pen.
    Code, § 13510, subd. (a); Cal. Code Regs, tit. 11, § 1005, subd. (a)), which teach
    interrogation techniques similar to those promoted by the Reid program. (Weisselberg,
    Mourning Miranda (2008) 96 Cal. L.Rev. 1519, 1533-1534 and Appen. (Mourning
    Miranda).)
    8
    “The first interrogation step is ‘a direct, positively presented confrontation of the
    suspect with a statement that he is considered to be the person who committed the
    offense.’ . . . [¶] The second step introduces a theme for the interrogation, a reason for
    the commission of the crime, which may be a moral (but not legal) excuse or a way for
    the suspect to rationalize her actions. . . . The suspect may deny involvement in the
    offense, which leads to step three, overcoming denials. . . . The next steps, four through
    six, guide the investigator in overcoming the suspect’s reasons why he would not or
    could not commit the crime, keeping the suspect’s attention and handling a suspect’s
    passive mood. [¶] Step seven is critical. Here the officer formulates alternative
    questions, one of which is ‘more “acceptable” or “understandable” than the other.’ The
    question is followed by a statement of support for the more morally acceptable
    alternative. However, ‘[w]hichever alternative is chosen by the suspect, the net effect . . .
    will be the functional equivalent of an incriminating admission.’ Steps eight and nine are
    taking the suspect’s oral statement and converting it to a written confession.” (Mourning
    
    Miranda, supra
    , 96 Cal. L.Rev. at pp. 1532-1533, fns. omitted.)
    12
    Although the Reid textbook states that “the purpose of an interrogation is to learn
    the truth,” 9 it makes clear that an interrogation should not be undertaken until after
    investigation points to the suspect’s likely guilt. The text emphasizes, “An interrogation
    is conducted only when the investigator is reasonably certain of the suspect’s guilt. The
    investigator should have some basis for believing a suspect has not told the truth before
    confronting the suspect. The basis for the belief may be the suspect’s behavior during an
    interview or inconsistencies within the suspect’s account, physical evidence, or
    circumstantial evidence, coupled with behavioral observations. Interrogations should not
    be used as a primary means to evaluate a suspect’s truthfulness; in most cases, that can be
    accomplished during a nonaccusatory interview.” (Inbau et al., Criminal 
    Interrogation, supra
    , at pp. 5-6.)
    Referring to the Reid text and other manuals that document interrogative
    procedures employed with success by law enforcement agencies, Miranda emphasized
    that “the ‘principal psychological factor contributing to a successful interrogation is
    privacy’ ” and that “ ‘the interrogation should take place in the investigator’s office or at
    least in a room of his choice.’ ” (
    Miranda, supra
    , 384 U.S. at p. 449.) A subject should
    not be interrogated in his own home, the manuals say, because there the subject is more
    likely to be “ ‘confident, indignant, or recalcitrant’ ”; and he “is more keenly aware of his
    rights and more reluctant to tell of his indiscretions of criminal behavior within the walls
    of his home. Moreover his family and other friends are nearby, their presence lending
    moral support. (Id. at pp. 449-450.) In a private place of his own choosing, “the
    investigator possesses all the advantages.” (Id. at p. 450.)
    9
    In response to the view of most behavioral scientists who study the subject that
    the purpose of interrogation is to induce confessions, the Reid text states: “A common
    misperception exists in believing that the purpose of an interrogation is to elicit a
    confession. Unfortunately, there are occasions when an innocent suspect is interrogated,
    and only after the suspect has been accused of committing the crime will his or her
    innocence become apparent. If the suspect can be eliminated based on his or her
    behavior or explanations offered during an interrogation, the interrogation must be
    considered successful because the truth was learned. Oftentimes an interrogation also
    will result in a confession, which again accomplishes the goal of learning the truth.”
    (Inbau et al., Criminal 
    Interrogation, supra
    , at p. 5.)
    13
    Detective Buchignani knew where Elias lived and agreed she could have spoken
    with him at his home, where at least one of his parents was likely to be present. She
    interrogated Elias at the school because she knew he would be there, knew the principal
    and had previously met with juveniles there, and “had time to take care of the case that
    day.” Elias was brought by the principal, the highest authority at the school, to a small
    room that normally accommodated two people, contained a single desk and three chairs,
    and “may” have had windows. Buchignani directed Elias to sit across from her and next
    to the principal; Sergeant Martinez stood behind Elias, and a third officer, a uniformed
    deputy, stood outside the door. As the Supreme Court has indicated, the mere fact of
    police questioning of a minor in the school-house setting may have a coercive effect,
    because the child’s “presence at school is compulsory and [his] disobedience at school is
    cause for disciplinary action.” 
    (J.D.B., supra
    , ___U.S. at p.___ [131 S.Ct. at p. 2405];
    see, Commonwealth v. Bell (Ky. 2012) 
    365 S.W.3d 216
    , 224-225; No 
    Match, supra
    , 38
    Hastings Const. L.Q. at p. 1073.)
    Like all contemporary police manuals on interrogation, those quoted in Miranda
    instruct that, in order “[t]o highlight the isolation and unfamiliar surroundings” in which
    the interrogation takes place, the police should “display an air of confidence in the
    suspect’s guilt and from outward appearance to maintain only an interest in confirming
    certain details. The guilt of the suspect is to be posited as a fact. The interrogator should
    direct his comments towards the reasons why the subject committed the act, rather than
    court failure by asking the subject whether he did it. Like other men, perhaps the subject
    has had a bad family life, had an unhappy childhood, had too much to drink, had an
    unrequited desire for women. The officers are instructed to minimize the moral
    seriousness of the offense, to cast blame on the victim or on society. These tactics are
    designed to put the subject in a psychological state where his story is but an elaboration
    of what the police purport to know already—that he is guilty. Explanations to the
    contrary are dismissed and discouraged.” (
    Miranda, supra
    , 348 U.S. at p. 450, fns.
    omitted.)
    The texts relied upon in Miranda also stressed the interrogator’s need for patience
    and perseverance, not just kindness and stratagems, because the investigator will
    14
    “ ‘encounter many situations where the sheer weight of his personality will be the
    deciding factor. Where emotional appeals and tricks are employed to no avail, he must
    rely on an oppressive atmosphere of dogged persistence. He must interrogate steadily
    and without relent, leaving the subject with no prospect of surcease. He must dominate
    his subject and overwhelm him with his inexorable will to obtain the truth. He should
    interrogate for a spell of several hours pausing only for the subject’s necessities in
    acknowledgment of the need to avoid a charge of duress that can be technically
    substantiated.’ ” (
    Miranda, supra
    , 384 U.S. at p. 451, fn. omitted.)
    Conforming to most of these directives, Detective Buchignani posited Elias’s guilt
    quickly and dispositively. Brushing off his repeated denials of her accusations,
    Buchignani told Elias he was “obviously nervous because you’re not telling me the truth
    about what happened.” Early in the interrogation Buchignani confidently declared that
    Elias’s improper touching of A.T. “did happen” because A.T. “explained it perfectly.”
    Ignoring Elias’s exculpatory or innocuous answers to questions regarding A.T.’s age, her
    brother, and exactly where Elias and A.T. were located on the bed, as well as his
    explanation that he simply unzipped A.T.’s pants when she asked him to because she
    wanted to change her clothes, Buchignani repeatedly referred to Elias’s guilt as an
    established fact and displayed interest only in confirming details, such as why and how
    Elias committed the act, and never allowing the possibility he may not have committed
    any unlawful act.
    Building on Elias’s statement that, while sitting on the edge of a bed playing a
    competitive video game with her brother, he turned “real fast” to help A.T. “unzip her
    pants cause she wanted to change,”10 and his acknowledgment that this involved touching
    “the outside of her,” Detective Buchignani’s questions, all insinuating Elias had
    improperly touched A.T.’s genitals, were relentless: “when her mom walked in, how
    come [A.T.’s] pants were down?”; “how come you ended up on the bed with her?”; “But
    her mom walked in and you were on the bed with her”; “how many fingers did you put
    inside her?”; “you touched the outside of her but you did not put fingers inside her?”;
    10
    Elias’s statement to Buchignani in this respect differs from Aurora’s testimony
    that he told her A.T. asked him to unzip her pants so she could go to the bathroom.
    15
    “you touched the outside of her”; “You were on the bed and you had her pants down. So
    the question again is how many fingers did you put inside of her?”; “So, you touched the
    outside of her?”; “You just touched the outside of her?”; “Okay, how long did you touch
    her?”; “So, your hand touched her bare vagina”; “So, I know that you touched her bare
    vagina and you know that you touched her bare vagina”; “But you put your fingers in her
    bare vagina”; “You’re okay with what you did?”; “You touched her”; “Why do you think
    you touched her”; “so you felt like you needed to touch her vagina when you were
    unzipping her pants?”; “Why were you kissing her before that whole incident?”; “How
    long have you been attracted to [A.T.]?”; “How long have you known that you have an
    attraction to her?”; “Why would you want to touch her?”; “You put your hand on her
    vagina?”; “What happens when we bring your sister in for an interview . . . and ask her
    what you do with her?”
    The aggressive nature and persistence of Buchignani’s questioning was part of an
    overall approach referred to in the literature on interrogation as “maximization/
    minimization,” a “cluster of tactics” designed to convey two things. The first is “the
    interrogator’s rock-solid belief that the suspect is guilty and that all denials will fail.
    Such tactics include making an accusation, overriding objections, and citing evidence,
    real or manufactured, to shift the suspects mental state from confidence to
    hopelessness. . . . In contrast, minimization tactics are to provide the suspect with moral
    justification and face saving excuses for having committed the crime in question,” a tactic
    that “communicates by implication that leniency in punishment is forthcoming upon
    confession.” (Police-Induced 
    Confessions, supra
    , 34 Law & Human Behav. at p. 12.) A
    convincing body of evidence demonstrates that implicit promises can put vulnerable
    innocents at risk to confess by encouraging them to think that the only way to lessen or
    escape punishment is compliance with the interrogator’s demand for confession,
    especially when minimization is used on suspects who are also led to believe that their
    continued denial is futile and prosecution inevitable. (Decision to Confess 
    Falsely, supra
    , 74 Denver U. L.Rev. 979; Gudjonsson, The Psychology of Interrogations,
    Confessions And Testimony (1992); Kassin & McCall, Police Interrogations and
    Confessions: Communicating Promises and Threats by Pragmatic Implication (1991) 15
    16
    Law & Human Behav. 233; Kassin & Keichel, The Social Psychology of False
    Confessions (1996) 7 Psych. 125; Police-Induced Confessions, at p. 12.)
    The maximization tactics Buchignani employed during her accusative questioning
    were deceptive in a variety of ways, including the use of false evidence. She told Elias
    the improper touching had happened because A.T. had “explained it perfectly,” and
    Aurora “walked in and saw” him touch A.T.’s vagina, though she knew that both
    representations were false and that neither Hector, who was present, nor anyone else,
    witnessed even the unzipping that Elias freely admitted.
    Studies demonstrate that the use of false evidence enhances the risk of false
    confessions. (Kassin, On the Psychology of Confessions: Does Innocence Put Innocents
    at Risk? (2005) 60 Am. Psychologist 215, 218.) “Confronting innocent people with false
    evidence—laboratory reports, fingerprints or footprints, eyewitness identification, failed
    polygraph tests—may cause them to disbelieve their own innocence or to confess falsely
    because they believe that police possess overwhelming evidence. Innocent suspects may
    succumb to despair and confess to escape the rigors of interrogation in the naïve belief
    that later investigation will establish their innocence rather than seek to confirm their
    guilt.” (Feld, Police Interrogation of Juveniles: An Empirical Study of Policy and
    Practice (2006) 97 J. Crim. Law & Criminology 219, 313, fns. omitted (Police
    Interrogation of Juveniles).)
    False evidence, the use of which is forbidden in Great Britain and many European
    nations (Police-Induced 
    Confessions, supra
    , 34 Law & Human Behav. at p. 17; Slobogin,
    An Empirically Based Comparison of American and European Regulatory Approaches to
    Police Investigation (2001) 22 Mich. J. Int’l. L. 423, 443-444), was used in many cases in
    this country in which defendants subsequently exonerated by DNA evidence were
    wrongfully convicted based upon confessions. (Police-Induced Confessions, at p. 17;
    False 
    Confessions, supra
    , 82 N.C.L.Rev. at p. 904) In the United States, although
    deceptive interrogation techniques do not necessarily invalidate incriminating statements
    (Frazier v. Cupp (1969) 
    394 U.S. 731
    , 739; People v. Smith (2007) 
    40 Cal. 4th 483
    , 505),
    they are relevant in considering the totality of the circumstances (Frazier, at p. 739) as “a
    factor which weighs against a finding of voluntariness [citations].” (People v. Hogan
    17
    (1982) 
    31 Cal. 3d 815
    , 840-841, overruled on other grounds in People v. Cooper (1991)
    
    53 Cal. 3d 771
    , 836.)
    Buchignani’s threat to subject Elias against his will to a lie detector test that would
    definitively reveal the falsity of his denials—referred to in the literature as “the lie
    detector ploy”—is among the most common interrogation techniques that result in false
    confessions. (See, e.g., Lykken, A Tremor in the Blood: Uses and Abuses of the Lie
    Detector (1981); The Decision to Confess 
    Falsely, supra
    , 74 Denver Univ. L.Rev. at
    pp. 1036-1041.)
    Toward the end of her interrogation Detective Buchignani shifted tactics from
    maximization to minimization, and it was clearly this strategy that finally induced Elias
    to make his inculpatory statements. Buchignani offered Elias two possible explanations
    for the sexual touching she asserted as fact, both of which she told him were completely
    “understandable”: that he acted on the basis of a natural “curiosity,” and that the act was
    one any normal person in his shoes would find “exciting.” As will be seen, offended by
    the suggestion he was “excited,” Elias agreed to the more acceptable alternative that he
    was merely “curious,” thereby admitting the felonious act. After Buchignani told Elias a
    lie detector “would come back deceptive because you’re lying,” insisted “[w]hy don’t we
    just get this over with and get this out there so we can get you the help you need,” and
    told Elias that “[y]our fingers touched [the] skin of her vagina,” Elias allowed, “Well, I
    didn’t like put my whole hand in there.” The remainder of the interrogation went this
    way:11
    “O: Okay, fair enough. So, I’m feeling pretty comfortable that you did not use
    your fingers to penetrate her vagina but I know that you touched her vagina with your
    hands, with your fingers. Okay? Why did you feel you wanted to do that? What did it
    feel like, was it exciting? Was it, what was it? People your age are curious that just
    happens sometimes. I understand that. Your body’s changing and things are happening
    to you. If you found it exciting then . . . .
    “EV: I didn’t find it exciting, and after that I found it was nasty.
    11
    “EV” is Elias V., “O” is Detective Buchignani.
    18
    “O: Okay. So after you touched her bare vagina with your fingers, you thought it
    was nasty?
    “EV: Yeah, like when I was smaller, my friend one time showed me her vagina
    and I thought it was disgusting.
    “O: Okay. So were you just curious when you touched her this time?
    “EV: Curious, well I’ve been curious about things.
    “O: Fair enough. Did you find it a little exciting?
    “EV: I think it’s disgusting.
    “O: But you did it so there must be something there. There must be some reason
    why you did it. You know, this happens sometimes. There’s a driving force that you
    need to deal with so the fact that you did it tells me that you found it exciting.
    “EV: I’m serious, I’m dead serious, I don’t find it exciting. I think it’s like kinda
    gross.
    “O: But you did it.
    “EV: For curiosity.
    “O: How long do you think you touched her?
    “EV: Well, I only touched her for like around 3 or 4 seconds because I was in the
    middle of a game.”
    By offering Elias alternative explanations for improperly touching A.T.—because
    he “found it exciting” or “out of curiosity”—Detective Buchignani employed a so-called
    “false choice” strategy. As stated in the Reid text, “[w]hen the investigator presents the
    alternative question to the suspect, it is not enough simply to ask the question and then
    wait for the suspect to answer. The investigator must encourage the suspect to select one
    of the two options. This is accomplished through the use of positive and negative
    ‘supporting statements.’ [¶] A positive supporting statement is one in which the
    investigator reinforces the belief that the correct choice is the one that seems to be
    morally excusable or at least one that represents a less socially revolting reason for
    committing the act. The investigator should state that if the positive alternative is true, it
    is something he can understand.” (Inbau et al., Criminal 
    Interrogation, supra
    , at pp. 298-
    299.)
    19
    After Elias admitted he touched A.T. out of curiosity for 3 or 4 seconds, Detective
    Buchignani told him he was describing a three-year-old “being a little flirtatious,” which
    was blaming her for something she was not capable of doing at that age, and pressed the
    point that Elias was “attracted to her . . . . Like attracted enough to touch her.” Getting
    nowhere with suggestions that Elias was sexually excited by A.T., Buchignani began to
    ask about Elias’s relationship with his sister. Elias denied any impropriety, adding, “I’m
    barely home. I’m always here playing basketball, the Teens Club, or . . . doing
    something like active, cause I’m an active person, I love sports.” At that point Elias
    appears to have begun sobbing, Detective Buchignani inquired whether he needed help,
    Elias responded “unintelligibly,” and Buchignani said that Elias’s father was on his way
    and she was “going to talk to him and explain the situation,” then take Elias into custody
    at the Juvenile Hall.
    Detective Buchignani’s accusatory interrogation was dominating, unyielding, and
    intimidating. We need not decide, however, whether this alone would suffice to
    undermine the voluntariness of Elias’s statements. Our finding that Elias’s statements
    were involuntary is based on a combination of factors: (1) Elias’s youth, which rendered
    him “ ‘most susceptible to influence’ [(Eddings v. Oklahoma (1982) 
    455 U.S. 104
    , 115)],
    and ‘outside pressures’ [(Roper v. Simmons (2005) 
    543 U.S. 551
    , 569).]” 
    (J.D.B., supra
    ,
    ___ U.S. at p. ___ [131 S.Ct. at p. 2405]; (2) the absence of any evidence corroborating
    Elias’s inculpatory statements and (3) the likelihood that Buchignani’s use of deception
    and overbearing tactics would induce involuntary and untrustworthy incriminating
    admissions.
    A.
    Chief Justice Warren’s analysis in Miranda pertains to the psychological
    techniques involved in the “active persuasion” commonly employed in the custodial
    interrogation of adults. There appears to be a growing consensus—among the supporters
    of those techniques, not just the critics—about the need for extreme caution in applying
    them to juveniles.
    As noted at the outset of our analysis, since Miranda courts have expressed
    growing concern that the pressures of custodial interrogation “ ‘can induce a
    20
    frighteningly high percentage of people to confess to crimes they never committed’ ”
    (Corley v. United 
    States, supra
    , 556 U.S. at pp. 320-321), and this concern is deepest in
    cases involving the custodial interrogation of juveniles. As recently stated in 
    J.D.B., supra
    , ___U.S. at p.___ [131 S.Ct. at p. 2403] “[a] child’s age is far ‘more than a
    chronological fact.’ [Citations.] It is a fact that ‘generates commonsense conclusions
    about behavior and perception.’ [Citation.] Such conclusions apply broadly to children
    as a class. And, they are self-evident to anyone who was a child once himself, including
    any police officer or judge. [¶] Time and again, this Court has drawn these
    commonsense conclusions for itself. We have observed that children ‘generally are less
    mature and responsible than adults,’ [citations]; that they ‘often lack the experience,
    perspective and judgment to recognize and avoid choices that could be detrimental to
    them,’ [citation]; that they ‘are more vulnerable or susceptible to . . . outside pressures’
    than adults [citations]; and so on. Addressing the specific context of police interrogation,
    we have observed that events that ‘would leave a man cold and unimpressed can overawe
    and overwhelm a lad in his early teens.’ [Citations.] Describing no one child in
    particular, these observations restate what ‘any parent knows’—indeed, what any person
    knows—about children generally. [Citation.]” (Ibid.; see also Gallegos v. Colorado
    (1962) 
    370 U.S. 49
    , 53 [“ ‘[a]ge 15 is a tender and difficult age for a boy . . . . He cannot
    be judged by the more exacting standards of maturity. That which would leave a man
    cold and unimpressed can overawe and overwhelm a lad in his early teens. This is the
    period of great instability which the crisis of adolescence produces’ ”].)
    Increasing awareness of the important differences between juveniles and adults in
    a variety of contexts (see, e.g., Roper v. 
    Simmons, supra
    , 543 U. S. at pp. 569-570 and
    cases there cited), and, more specifically, the realization that children and adolescents are
    much more vulnerable to psychologically coercive interrogations and in other dealings
    with the police than resilient adults experienced with the criminal justice, is not limited to
    the courts; it is now far more widely shared by police manuals than when Miranda was
    decided.
    Thus, for example, the most recent edition of the Reid manual on interrogations
    notes that although the use of deception, including the use of “fictitious evidence which
    21
    implicates the subject,” has been upheld by the courts (see, e.g. Frazier v. 
    Cupp, supra
    ,
    394 U.S. at p. 739; People v. 
    Smith, supra
    , 40 Cal.4th at p. 505), “this technique should
    be avoided when interrogating a youthful suspect with low social maturity” because such
    suspects “may not have the fortitude or confidence to challenge such evidence and
    depending on the nature of the crime, may become confused as to their own possible
    involvement if the police tell them evidence clearly indicates they committed the crime.
    Factors such as the adolescent’s level of social responsibility and general maturity should
    be considered before fictitious evidence is introduced.” (Inbau et al., Criminal
    
    Interrogation, supra
    , at p. 255.)
    The developing consensus about the dangers of interrogation has resulted from the
    growing number of studies showing that the risk interrogation will produce a false
    confession is significantly greater for juveniles than for adults; indeed, juveniles usually
    account for one-third of proven false confession cases. (See, e.g., Susceptibility of
    
    Juveniles, supra
    , 62 Rutgers L.Rev. at p. 952; Police Interrogation of 
    Juveniles, supra
    ,
    97 J. Crim. L. & Criminology 219, False 
    Confessions, supra
    , 82 N.C.L.Rev. at p. 944;
    Gross et al., Exonerations in the United States, 1989 through 2003, 95 J. Crim. L. &
    Criminology 523, 545; Redlich & Kassin, Police Interrogation and False Confessions:
    The Inherent Risk of Youth, in Children as Victims, Witnesses, and Offenders:
    Psychological Science and the Law (2009) 275-276.
    A 2007 study of 332 law enforcement officers showed that, despite police
    acknowledgment of the fallibility of deception detection and differences in typical
    behaviors of children and youth in comparison to adults, an average of 83.2 percent of
    police claimed to use body language to detect deception, without discrimination of the
    age of the subject. (Meyer & Reppucci, Police Practices and Perceptions Regarding
    Juvenile Interrogation and Interrogative Suggestibility, 25 Behavioral Sciences & the
    Law 757.) As the authors of the study point out, “[t]his is especially dangerous because
    police are trained to view behaviors such as slouching and lack of eye contact as signals
    of deception and indications that they should proceed with interrogations. If police
    perceive these typical juvenile behaviors as deceptive, this perception may increase the
    frequency with which they judge young suspects to be guilty, therefore increasing the
    22
    frequency with which they subject youth to coercive and deceptive interrogation to obtain
    a confession.”12 (Id. at pp. 774-775.) The study concludes that “police acknowledge
    some developmental age differences concerning comprehension abilities, but fail to apply
    this knowledge to the interrogation context.” (Id. at p. 774.) One of the findings of the
    study particularly pertinent to this case is that “a fair number of police (21.4%) endorsed
    usage of verbally tricky, forced choice questions where either choice incriminates the
    subject, without discrimination of the age of the suspect, indicating little knowledge or
    application [and knowledge of factual findings] that children are likely to choose between
    the forced-choice answers presented by the police even when none are correct.” (Ibid.)
    Contrary to the trial court’s apparent view that Detective Buchignani did not
    utilize this technique,13 her repetitive queries whether Elias touched A.T. “out of
    curiosity” or because it was “exciting” were precisely the sort of forced-choice question
    that can easily induce an adolescent such as Elias to falsely incriminate himself when
    confronted with false evidence of his guilt.
    In a case with some striking similarities to the one before us—not least the
    apparent use of interrogation techniques drawing on the Reid methodology—a Kentucky
    court emphasized the significance of the 13-year-old suspect’s age in evaluating the
    effect on him of various aspects of the questioning. (Commonwealth v. 
    Bell, supra
    , 365
    S.W.3d at pp. 224-225.) Like Elias, T.C. was questioned at his middle school by two
    police detectives who had school officials remove the boy from class and bring him to a
    separate room for questioning about allegations that he had anal intercourse with his six-
    year-old cousin in the shower. (Id. at p. 219.) The detective told T.C. that “thirteen-year-
    old boys ‘have a lot of hormones,’ and sometimes get ‘horny’ and ‘get a little bit
    curious,’ ” then asked what had happened in the shower. (Ibid.) As T.C. denied
    12
    The similarity between the postures common to juveniles and those deemed by
    many police manuals to be indicative of deceptiveness is powerfully illustrated by a
    series of photographs set forth in chapter 9 of the most recent edition of the interrogation
    manual, entitled “Behavior Symptom Analysis” (Inbau et al., Criminal 
    Interrogation, supra
    , at pp. 123-128, figures 9-8 through 9-13).
    13
    As indicated above, the court noted that the detective’s “questions weren’t split
    where there would be two responses you’d have to use to the same single question.”
    23
    improper conduct, the detective insisted he already knew what happened but wanted T.C.
    to be honest with him; said he had to find out whether it happened accidentally or
    intentionally; suggested T.C. might have been curious or might have been “messing
    around”; insisted he needed to know why T.C. did it; and told T.C. he had to be honest
    and “[w]e can be done here.” (Id. at p. 220.)14 Finally, the detective said, “ ‘you did it
    because you were horny, had a hard on, and you were curious. . . . Am I right?’ ” (Ibid.)
    T.C. replied, “yes, sir.” (Ibid.)
    Upholding the lower court’s finding that the confession was involuntary, the Bell
    court noted the prosecution’s argument that the detectives did not deprive T.C. of food or
    sleep and used a calm, conversational tone. But, the court explained: “These latter
    statements may serve to assure an adult, or even a mature minor, that he should feel free
    of coercion, that he is free to say nothing and even to leave the officers’ presence any
    time he desires. However, we do not believe they provided that same assurance, under
    these circumstances, to this thirteen-year-old boy.” (Commonwealth v. 
    Bell, supra
    , 365
    S.W.3d at p. 224.) “[A] school is where compliance with adult authority is required and
    where such compliance is compelled almost exclusively by the force of authority. Like it
    or not, that is the definition of coercion. . . . If he is sent to the principal’s office, he is
    not allowed to leave until the principal says so. And if he is instructed to be alone in a
    room with police detectives, as T.C. was, how can we expect him to believe some other
    set of rules applies? Can we reasonably expect a thirteen-year-old child to perceive he
    14
    When T.C. denied anything had happened in the shower, the detective said, “ ‘I
    know what happened in the shower. I just want you to be honest with me.’ ”
    (Commonwealth v. 
    Bell, supra
    , 365 S.W.3d at p. 219.) The detective told T.C. that the
    cousin claimed “he was ‘bent over and [T.C.’s] penis went in [his cousin’s] butt.’ ”
    When T.C. reiterated his denial, the officer said he knew something had happened and
    could not leave until he knew whether it happened accidentally or intentionally. (Ibid.)
    T.C. then said his cousin was playing in the shower and fell back onto T.C., and T.C.’s
    penis went “around” his cousin’s butt but not into it. (Ibid.) The detective asked if T.C.
    was “curious to see what it felt like” and, in response to T.C.’s denial, insisted that T.C.’s
    penis “went in his butt” and asked T.C. why he did it, then offered possible scenarios:
    “ ‘[Y]ou did it because you were either curious or you did it because you were messing
    around, poking at him.’ ” (Id. at p. 220.) Questioning T.C. about why he did it, the
    detective said, “ ‘the one thing I gotta break through here is why you did it. You gotta
    tell me that honestly. You gotta be honest. We can be done here.’ ” (Ibid.)
    24
    has greater freedom while in school simply because he was read his Miranda rights?
    When the detective said, ‘I really can’t leave here until I find out’ something, is it
    reasonable to believe T.C. did not feel coerced into saying something, whether true or
    not; is it reasonable that he believed he had the right to say nothing or to get up and leave
    the detective there alone? We believe not. [¶] Although the thirty-two minute
    interrogation may not seem excessive, the repetitive questioning amounted to coercion by
    importunity. T.C., alone, was ordered by school officials into a room, facing adult
    authority figures with considerable power, who also feigned superior knowledge (‘I know
    what happened [and your cousin] has not lied to me about anything’), and who repeatedly
    demanded answers that he, if he was to be an obedient child, would have to provide.
    How could T.C. not perceive such a situation as subjectively coercive?” (Id. at p. 225,
    italics added, fn. omitted.) “T.C. was an impressionable youth inclined to acquiesce to
    coercive police tactics. . . . In sum, viewing the interrogation through the lens of this
    thirteen-year-old student, under these circumstances, we are persuaded the district court
    did not err in finding T.C.’s statements to Detective Johnson ‘were not the product of
    [his] free choice’ when given.” (Ibid.)
    At 13 years of age, Elias was a young adolescent, there is no indication in the
    record he was particularly sophisticated, and he had no prior confrontations with the
    police. Buchignani interrogated him in a small room at his school, with the school
    principal and a second officer present, and another officer outside the door. There is
    every reason to believe the aggressive, deceptive, and unduly suggestive tactics
    Buchignani employed would have been particularly intimidating in these circumstances.
    B.
    As we have said, aside from Elias’s interrogation, the only evidence that Elias
    touched A.T. in an improper manner was Aurora’s statement the child told her he did so
    and the detective’s testimony that in the RCC interview A.T. said Elias “touched her”
    and, in the detective’s opinion, pointed to the vaginal area on a doll. Aurora did not see
    Elias touch A.T.; nor did Hector, who was playing the video game with Elias.15 During
    15
    Hector testified that he was sitting on the floor and Elias on the bed; Elias stated
    in the interrogation that Hector was “right beside me.”
    25
    the 17 days between the occurrence of the alleged act and Aurora’s complaint to the
    police, A.T. must have interacted with her brother, father, uncles and other members and
    friends of the family frequently at the apartment, as well as residents of the apartment
    house, yet there is no evidence anyone other than Aurora heard the things Aurora claimed
    her daughter had been “telling everybody.” Although A.T. was interviewed, little
    evidence derived from that interview was introduced. The sole evidence of the manner in
    which Elias allegedly touched the child came from Elias’s interrogation, and no evidence
    corroborated his incriminating statements.
    The best form of corroboration is the suspect’s revelation of information only a
    guilty suspect would know. (Inbau et al., Criminal 
    Interrogation, supra
    , at pp. 354-356.)
    Thus “[t]he admissions, ‘I shot and killed Mr. Johnson’ or ‘I forced Susie Adams to have
    sex with me’ may be elicited from a juvenile (or adult) suspect. These admissions
    become useful as evidence if they are corroborated by (1) information about the crime the
    suspect provides which was purposefully withheld from the suspect, and/or, (2)
    information not known by the police until after the confession which is subsequently
    verified.” (Id. at p. 255.) Corroboration is “[t]he ultimate test of the trustworthiness of a
    confession.” (Ibid.) A suspect saying “ ‘I did it’ ” does not provide assurance that the
    admission is true; “internal indicia of reliability and independent evidence” are necessary.
    (Decision to Confess 
    Falsely, supra
    , 74 Denv. U. L.Rev. at pp. 990-991.)
    As we will explain, post, at pages 29-30, Elias’s admissions did not even amount
    to an “I did it.” Internal indicia of reliability were absent, as Elias said nothing during his
    interrogation that only a guilty suspect would know. Rather, all of the differing
    descriptions of where and how the alleged improper touching took place were first
    offered by Detective Buchignani.
    One of the ways police facilitate false confessions is by disclosing specific facts
    regarding the crime during the interrogation process, inducing the suspect to adopt these
    facts and thus accurately “confirm[] the preconceived story the police seek to have him
    describe.” (
    Miranda, supra
    , 384 U.S. at p. 455; Nirider et al., Combating Contamination
    in Confession Cases (2012) 79 U. Chi. L.Rev. 837, 847 (Combating Contamination).)
    The use of this suggestive technique—referred to as “contamination” (see Combating
    26
    Contamination, at pp. 846-847); Garrett, Contaminated Confessions Revisited (2015) 101
    U.Va. L.Rev. 395, has been found to be coercive and to have overcome the will of
    subjects, particularly those who are young or otherwise vulnerable. (See, e.g., United
    States v. Preston (9th Cir. 2014) 
    751 F.3d 1008
    , 1023-1024 (en banc); In re J.F. (D.C.
    2010) 
    987 A.2d 1168
    ; State v. Rettenberger (Utah 1999) 
    984 P.2d 1009
    , 1020; Passama
    v. Nevada (Nev. 1987) 
    735 P.2d 321
    , 324.) This is why investigators are told not to
    reveal to suspects all information they have about the crime: Revelation to a suspect of
    known details about a crime is discouraged because it creates the possibility a confession
    may simply reflect suggestions made by the investigator, “not the product of spontaneous
    recall on the part of the suspect.” (Inbau et al., Criminal 
    Interrogation, supra
    , at p. 359;
    see Combating Contamination, at p. 847.)
    Contamination also “prevents police from testing and corroborating the reliability
    of the admissions and confessions they elicit.” (Combating 
    Contamination, supra
    , 79 U.
    Chicago L.Rev. at p. 848.) As one of the authors of Criminal Interrogation has said, “[I]t
    is imperative that interrogators do not reveal details of the crime so that they can use the
    disclosure of such information by the suspect as verification of the confession’s
    authenticity. In each case there should be documented ‘hold back’ information about the
    details of how the crime was committed; details from the crime scene; details about
    specific activities perpetrated by the offender; etc. The goal is to match the suspect’s
    confession against these details to establish the veracity of the statement.” (Combating
    Contamination, at pp. 847-848, quoting Joseph P. Buckley, The Reid Technique of
    Interviewing and Interrogation, in Tom Williamson ed., Investigative Interviewing:
    Rights, Research, Regulation 190, 204-05 (Willan 2005).)
    Here, because Buchignani suggested all of the scenarios involving improper
    touching, she enhanced the likelihood that Elias’s statements resulted from his
    suggestibility rather than recall of actual events. Further, because there was no
    evidentiary basis for the suggestions, any contamination that occurred necessarily
    resulted in Elias’s adoption of facts that were entirely speculative on Buchignani’s part.
    And because Buchignani had no information about the alleged offense, she had no basis
    for evaluating the veracity of Elias’s statements. As a result, not only are Elias’s
    27
    inculpatory admissions wholly uncorroborated, they leave open the real possibility that
    Elias simply accepted a description of the events—that he briefly touched the bare skin of
    A.T.’s vagina—that was deceptively suggested by Detective Buchignani.
    C.
    Use of the deceptive techniques employed by Detective Buchignani, which are
    essentially those critically described in Miranda, “is a factor which weighs against a
    finding of voluntariness [citations].” (People v. 
    Hogan, supra
    , 31 Cal.3d at pp. 840-841.)
    And, given the vulnerabilities we have discussed, the use of deceptive techniques is
    significantly more indicative of involuntariness where, as here, the subject is a 13-year-
    old adolescent who has never previously had any confrontation with the police.
    The authors of the text expounding the Reid Technique candidly admit that
    “[m]any of the interrogation techniques presented in this text involve duplicity and
    pretense. To persuade a guilty suspect to offer an admission against self-interest, the
    investigator may have to falsely exaggerate confidence in the suspect’s guilt, sympathize
    with the suspect’s situation, and display feelings toward the suspect or his crime that are
    far from genuine. The investigator may suggest a face-saving motive for the commission
    of the crime, knowing it is not true. In some cases an investigator may falsely imply, or
    outright state, that evidence exists that links the suspect to the crime.” (Inbau et al.,
    Criminal 
    Interrogation, supra
    , at p. 351.) But, as we have said, the text makes it
    eminently clear that such deceptive techniques “should be avoided when interrogating a
    youthful suspect with low social maturity” because such suspects “may not have the
    fortitude or confidence to challenge such evidence” and “may become confused as to
    their own possible involvement, if the police tell them evidence clearly indicates they
    committed the crime.” (Id. at p. 352, italics added.)
    Indeed, our review of the interrogation leaves considerable question what Elias
    meant to be saying when he made the statements that incriminated him, or whether he
    understood their significance. As we have described, Elias repeatedly stated that all he
    did was help A.T. unzip her pants at her request. He “touched” her, yes—because it
    would be impossible to unzip her pants without touching her: “I did touch her but I never
    really touched her like go like that or do something to her. [¶] . . . [¶] I wasn’t touching
    28
    her vagina.” When Buchignani insisted that Elias touched the skin of A.T.’s vagina, he
    said he “didn’t like put my whole hand in there” and, later, that his “finger like slipped,
    but I did touch it but I didn’t put it in there and just leave it”; when asked how long he
    touched A.T., he said, “I only touched her for like around 3 or 4 seconds because I was in
    the middle of a game.” These responses leave very unclear whether Elias understood
    himself to be acknowledging that he “touched” A.T. in the way Buchignani meant rather
    than that he “touched” her to unzip her pants in a few-seconds interruption of the video
    game he was in the midst of playing with Hector. At one point, Elias having denied
    finding the touching “exciting” and said “after that I thought it was nasty,” Buchignani
    asked him, “[s]o, after you touched her bare vagina with your fingers you though[t] it was
    nasty?” Elias responded, “Yeah, like when I was smaller, my friend one time showed me
    her vagina and I thought it was disgusting.” The answer is ambiguous: Perhaps Elias
    was referring to another time when he thought it was disgusting just like he did this time,
    but perhaps he was simply responding to Buchignani’s questions about vaginas by
    relating something that had happened in the past. If Elias did not understand that his
    answers conveyed an admission that he touched A.T. improperly, it would make no sense
    to view them as voluntary and a product of his free will.
    The cases respondent relies upon to argue the interrogation tactics used here were
    not inappropriate are distinguishable in critical ways. The only one of these cases
    involving a juvenile is In re Joe R. (1980) 
    27 Cal. 3d 496
    . The minor, nearly 18 years old,
    denied participation in two robberies for the first 30 to 40 minutes of interrogation, then
    confessed after the interrogating officer accused him of lying “loudly, emphatically, and
    with terse language (e.g., ‘bullshit’)” and confronted him with incriminating evidence
    that had been discovered in the closet of his bedroom. (Id. at pp. 502-503, 515) The
    Supreme Court upheld the trial court’s determination that the confession was voluntary,
    explaining that “ ‘[m]ere advice or exhortation by the police that it would be better for the
    accused to tell the truth when unaccompanied by either a threat or a promise does not
    render a subsequent confession involuntary’ ” and the trial court “had no duty to rule that
    loud, aggressive accusations of lying amounted to coercive threats.” (Id. at p. 515,
    quoting People v. Jimenez (1978) 
    21 Cal. 3d 595
    , 611, overruled on other grounds in
    29
    People v. Cahill (1993) 
    5 Cal. 4th 478
    , 484, 510.) The Supreme Court also noted that the
    confession was not invalidated by “ ‘deception . . . of a type reasonably likely to procure
    an untrue statement.’ ” (Ibid., quoting In re Walker (1974) 
    10 Cal. 3d 764
    , 777.) Thus, In
    re Joe R. involved a minor significantly older than Elias, the interrogator did not use false
    evidence, false choice questions or other forms of deception, and the interrogation was
    preceded by an investigation that independently provided substantial inculpatory and
    corroborating evidence.
    Illinois v. Perkins (1990) 
    496 U.S. 292
    , which respondent relies upon for the point
    that police use of deception is not necessarily coercive, involved neither a juvenile nor an
    interrogation: The defendant’s statements were made to an undercover agent posing as a
    fellow jail inmate. The court held that the premise of Miranda was “that the danger of
    coercion results from the interaction of custody and official interrogation” and “[t]he
    essential ingredients of a ‘police-dominated atmosphere’ and compulsion are not present
    when an incarcerated person speaks freely to someone [whom] he believes to be a fellow
    inmate.” (Illinois, at pp. 296-297.)
    Respondent’s other authorities state the proposition that misrepresentation by the
    police does not necessarily invalidate a confession. (Amaya-Ruiz v. Stewart (9th Cir.
    1997) 
    121 F.3d 486
    [misrepresentation inflated extent of witness evidence], overruled on
    other grounds in United States v. 
    Preston, supra
    , 751 F.3d at pp. 1019-1020; Pollard v.
    Galaza (9th Cir. 2002) 
    290 F.3d 1030
    , 1034 [no misrepresentation]; People v. Maury
    (2003) 
    30 Cal. 4th 342
    , 411 [defendant recognized ruse employed by Department of
    Corrections psychologist and refused to succumb to deception]; People v. Jones (1998)
    
    17 Cal. 4th 279
    , 299 [detective implied he knew more than he did or could prove more
    than he could].) As none of these cases involve juveniles or are otherwise factually
    similar to the present case, none are helpful in evaluating the effect of the use of false
    evidence here.
    “[R]esearch on juveniles’ ability to exercise Miranda rights and their adjudicative
    competence consistently reports that, as a group, adolescents understand legal
    proceedings and make decisions less well than do adults. Youths fifteen years of age and
    younger exhibited the clearest and greatest disability.” (Police Interrogation of
    30
    
    Juveniles, supra
    , 97 J. Crim. L. & Criminology at p. 233; Grisso, Juveniles’ Waivers of
    Rights; Legal and Psychological Competence (1980); Grisso, Juveniles’ Capacities to
    Waive Miranda Rights: An Empirical Analysis (1980) 68 Cal. L.Rev. 1134.) “Social
    expectations of obedience to authority and children’s lower social status make them more
    vulnerable than adults during interrogation. Less powerful people, such as juveniles or
    racial minorities, often speak indirectly with authority figures to avoid provoking
    conflict. Juveniles may acquiesce more readily to police suggestions during
    questioning.” (Police Interrogation of Juveniles, at p. 230, fns. omitted; Ferguson &
    Douglas, A Study of Juvenile Waiver (1970) 7 San Diego L.Rev. 39; Grisso & Pomiciter,
    Interrogation of Juveniles: An Empirical Study of Procedures, Safeguards, and Rights
    Waiver (1977) 1 Law & Hum. Behav. 321; Drizin & Colgan, Tales From the Juvenile
    Confession Front: A Guide to How Standard Police Interrogation Tactics Can Produce
    Coerced and False Confessions from Juvenile Suspects, in Interrogations, Confessions,
    and Entrapment (Lassiter ed., 2004) 153-155.) Given their vulnerabilities to
    interrogation, the inordinate number of false confessions given by juveniles should come
    as no surprise.
    The false evidence and other deceptive techniques employed in this case to induce
    a 13-year-old adolescent to incriminate himself create substantial doubt about both the
    voluntariness of Elias’s inculpatory statements and the truth of those statements—even
    though Elias need not show they were not trustworthy. (Rogers v. 
    Richmond, supra
    , 365
    U.S. at pp. 543-544). These doubts must be resolved adversely to the prosecution due to
    both the absence of any evidence corroborating the truth of Elias’s incriminating
    statements, which we have discussed, and the presence of evidence suggesting they may
    be false. As we have explained, the record provides significant reason to think Aurora’s
    claims may have been motivated by a desire to retaliate against Elias’s father for
    complaining to the landlord and the police about her family.
    Additionally, although we do not assign it great weight, we are troubled by the fact
    that, almost immediately after the trial court’s jurisdictional ruling against him, Elias told
    a probation officer that he did not commit the adjudicated offense but “only tried to help
    the young girl.” As noted in the Reid text, “[a]s soon as the threat of the interrogation has
    31
    been removed, it would be expected that the innocent suspect would denounce the
    confession and protest innocence to anyone willing to listen.” (Inbau et al., Criminal
    
    Interrogation, supra
    , at p. 357.)
    In finding Elias’s statements voluntary and admissible, the trial court stated that
    Detective Buchignani’s manner was “gentle” and “calm,” her questions were “not
    convoluted,” her language was age appropriate, and “the questions weren’t split where
    there would be two responses you’d have to use the same single question.” As we have
    explained, the court’s last point, that Buchignani did not employ a “false choice strategy,”
    was clearly mistaken. But this inattention, and the superficiality of all of the other factors
    the trial court also relied upon, demonstrate the need for much more rigorous judicial
    scrutiny of voluntariness of statements made under the “inherently compelling pressures”
    of the forms of custodial interrogation now in common use by law enforcement agencies.
    Many who study the subject closely think post-Miranda police practices “have gutted
    Miranda’s safeguards” so that today, “Miranda’s protections are more mythic than real.”
    (See, e.g., Mourning 
    Miranda, supra
    , 96 Cal. L.Rev. at p. 1599; Kamisar, On the Fortieth
    Anniversary of the Miranda Case: Why We Needed It, How We Got It – And What
    Happened to It (2007) 5 Ohio St. J. of Crim. L. 163; White, False Confessions and the
    Constitution: Safeguards Against Untrustworthy Confessions (1997) 32 Harv. C.R.-C.L.
    L.Rev. 105.) Increasingly, the survival of those protections, and the vindication of the
    Miranda court’s concern about the increasing number of false confessions, which is of
    particular concern with adolescent suspects, may depend upon the willingness of trial
    judges to engage in vigorous individual assessment of the voluntariness of a statement
    despite the suspect’s Miranda waiver. As Justice Souter has remarked, “giving the
    warnings and getting a waiver has generally produced a virtual ticket of admissibility;
    maintaining that a statement is involuntary even though given after warnings and
    voluntary waiver of rights requires unusual stamina, and litigation over voluntariness
    tends to end with the finding of a valid waiver.” (Missouri v. Seibert (2004) 
    542 U.S. 600
    , 608-609 (plur. opn.).) The fact of a Miranda waiver should not be allowed to serve
    as an obstacle to the vital assessment of voluntariness. (Garcia, Is Miranda Dead, Was it
    Overruled or Is It Irrelevant? (1998) 10 St. Thomas L.Rev. 461, 496-502.)
    32
    In the present case, considering the totality of the circumstances and the
    vulnerabilities and susceptibility of adolescents subjected to custodial interrogation that
    have been emphasized by the Supreme Court 
    (J.D.B., supra
    , ___U.S. at p. ___ [131 S.Ct.
    at p. 2403], and cases there cited), we have little difficulty concluding that the inculpatory
    statements made by Elias to Detective Buchignani cannot be deemed a product of his free
    will.
    D.
    Although it may not bear directly on the voluntariness of Elias’s statements, one of
    the most remarkable features of the present case is that before interrogating Elias,
    Detective Buchignani had neither interviewed him or anyone else about this case nor
    investigated or questioned the truth of Aurora’s statement to Officer Chavez that she
    “believed” Elias “had assaulted her daughter” on October 6th. This point is significant
    because proceeding in this manner violated a basic tenet of the Reid Technique meant to
    reduce the likelihood of inducing false confessions.
    So far as the record shows, the only police conduct prior to the interrogation that
    could be described as investigatory was the short statement Officer Chavez took from
    Aurora, and the apparently uninformative interview of A.T. Aurora did not tell Chavez
    she saw Elias touch A.T., and she said her 8-year-old son Hector had told her he “didn’t
    witness anything.” The trained expert who interviewed A.T. never testified and the tape
    of the interview was never introduced in evidence, as it surely would have been if A.T.’s
    statements or behavior had corroborated Aurora’s statements to the police. There was
    only Buchignani’s testimony that A.T. gave “enough information to believe she had been
    touched” by, as it appeared to Buchignani, pointing to the vaginal area on a doll.
    Buchignani confirmed at the jurisdictional hearing that she had never spoken with Elias
    prior to interrogating him and had never previously spoken with anyone else in Elias’s or
    A.T.’s family, the landlord, any tenant of the apartment house, any student or teacher or
    other authority at Elias’s school, or anyone else who might shed light on the case. As
    Buchignani testified, her agency “didn’t make contact with anyone other than [Aurora].”
    As we have said, it is accepted by virtually all respected authorities on custodial
    interrogation that aggressive interrogation designed to overwhelm the subject with the
    33
    interrogator’s indomitable will to obtain the truth is a technique that should only be used
    after an investigation has indicated that “ ‘the guilt of the subject appears highly
    probable.’ ” (
    Miranda, supra
    , 384 U.S. at p. 451, fn. omitted.) The significance of this
    principle arises from the distinctive characteristics of an interrogation that differentiate it
    from an ordinary investigative interview.
    As underscored in the opening pages of the current edition of the text expounding
    the Reid Technique, an “interview” is “nonaccusatory,” its purpose “is to gather
    information,” “it may be conducted early in an investigation,” “it may be conducted in a
    variety of environments,” the conversation should be “free flowing and relatively
    unstructured,” and “the investigator should take written notes.” (Inbau et al., Criminal
    
    Interrogation, supra
    , at pp. 3-4.) On the other hand, an “interrogation” is “accusatory”
    and “involves active persuasion,” it “is conducted in a controlled environment” and “only
    when the investigator is reasonably certain of the suspect’s guilt,” and the investigator
    “should not take any notes until after the suspect has told the truth and is fully committed
    to that position.” (Id. at pp. 5-6, italics added.)
    Proponents of the Reid Technique, and virtually all interrogation manuals, counsel
    that interrogation should almost never be undertaken without the benefit of a previous
    interview: “Absent a life-saving circumstance the investigator should conduct a non-
    accusatory interview before engaging in any interrogation. During the interview the
    investigator can establish rapport with the suspect, assess their credibility, develop
    investigative information and establish a behavioral baseline. Also, during the interview
    the suspect is more likely to reveal information that can be used to develop an
    interrogation strategy.”16
    While Aurora’s report of what her three-year-old daughter told her undoubtedly
    warranted investigation, including an interview of Elias, Detective Buchignani moved too
    quickly to interrogate him in a manner unjustified even by the standards of the Reid
    Technique itself. “The message of interrogation in the pre-admission phase is that the
    16
    The Reid Technique of Interviewing and Interrogation-Position Paper, available
    at  (as of June 1, 2015); see Inbau et
    al., Criminal 
    Interrogation, supra
    , at pp. 6-7.)
    34
    evidence already in hand leads to the conclusion that the suspect’s factual guilt has been
    established beyond any doubt. Although sometimes it is possible to be entirely truthful
    when making this claim, it is permissible for the claim to be a complete lie. By forcefully
    presenting a claim that he knows is entirely fabricated to someone he guesses is guilty,
    the investigator hopes to create the impression of an airtight case and convince the
    suspect that resistance is futile. When real evidence is lacking the investigator knows full
    well that it is only by getting the suspect to give a full and detailed confession that he will
    be able to find the evidence that will prove correct his speculative, intuitive, and risky
    guess.” (The Decision to Confess 
    Falsely, supra
    , 74 Denver Univ. L.Rev. at p. 1008, fns.
    omitted.)
    Against the advice of all police manuals and other authorities, Detective
    Buchignani used custodial interrogation as not just the primary but the only means of
    evaluating Elias’s truthfulness. As noted by a prominent student of custodial
    interrogation, innocent people falsely accused often believe in “a just world and in the
    transparency of their own blameless status . . . . [T]hose who stand falsely accused also
    have faith that their innocence will become self-evident to others. As a result they
    cooperate with police, often not realizing that they are suspects, not witnesses; they waive
    their rights to silence, counsel, and a lineup; they agree to take lie-detector tests; they
    vehemently protest their innocence, unwittingly triggering aggressive interrogation
    behavior; and they succumb to pressures to confess when isolated, trapped by false
    evidence, and offered hope via minimization and the leniency it implies. Yet without
    independent exculpatory evidence, their innocence is not easily detected by others.” (On
    the Psychology of 
    Confessions, supra
    , 60 Am. Psychologist at p. 224, italics added.) The
    “lesson of history,” our Supreme Court has observed, is “that a system of criminal law
    enforcement which comes to depend on the ‘confession’ will, in the long run, be less
    reliable and more subject to abuses than a system which depends on extrinsic evidence
    independently secured through skillful investigation.” (Escobedo v. Illinois (1964) 
    378 U.S. 478
    , 489-490, fns. omitted.)
    The voluntariness of inculpatory statements made during an interrogation
    conducted on the basis of no more than the interrogator’s “speculative, intuitive, and
    35
    risky guess” that the subject is guilty warrants particularly careful judicial scrutiny. A
    confession resulting from an interrogation undertaken in the absence of evidence strongly
    indicative of guilt is not necessarily inadmissible, but it is a circumstance to be carefully
    considered in evaluating the voluntariness of the resulting confession. And, of course,
    strict scrutiny of uncorroborated confessions elicited during such interrogations may also
    have the salutary effect of discouraging an unprofessional and hazardous police practice.
    E.
    We conclude that the prosecution failed to prove by a preponderance of the
    evidence that Elias’s inculpatory statements were voluntary, and the trial court therefore
    erred in receiving the statements in evidence. Because the jurisdictional ruling was based
    almost entirely on those statements, the error was prejudicial. Accordingly, the judgment
    must be and is reversed.
    36
    _________________________
    Kline, P.J.
    We concur:
    _________________________
    Richman, J.
    _________________________
    Miller, J.
    Trial Court:                                Sonoma County Superior Court
    Trial Judge:                                Hon. Raima Ballinger
    Attorneys for Defendant and Appellant:      Under appointment by the First
    District Court of Appeal
    L. Richard Braucher
    Attorneys for Amicus Curiae on behalf of    Center on Wrongful Convictions of Youth
    Defendant and Appellant:                    Megan G. Crane
    Joshua A. Tepfer
    Attorneys for Plaintiff and Respondent:     Attorney General of California
    Kamala D. Harris
    Gerald A. Engler
    Senior Assistant Attorney General
    Eric D. Share
    Supervising Deputy Attorney General
    Sharon G. Birenbaum
    Deputy Attorney General
    37